You are viewing the site in preview mode

Skip to main content

Predicting psychotic-like experiences among adolescents: the interplay of childhood trauma, cognitive biases, neuroticism, and depression

Abstract

Background

Childhood trauma, cognitive biases, neuroticism, and depression have emerged as crucial risk markers for psychotic-like experiences (PLEs). However, the interplay among these variables in influencing the risk of PLEs remains largely unexplored. This study aims to investigate the effect of the complex relationship between childhood trauma, cognitive biases, neuroticism, and depression on the risk of PLEs among adolescents.

Methods

A total of 4,087 adolescents from three senior high schools were recruited for this study. We utilized the Community Assessment of Psychic Experiences to measure PLEs, the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire to assess childhood trauma, the Davos Assessment of Cognitive Biases scale to evaluate cognitive biases, the neuroticism subscale of the Neuroticism-Extraversion-Openness Personality Inventory to assess neuroticism, and the Patient Health Questionnaire to measure depression. Psychiatric diagnoses were screened using a self-report questionnaire.

Results

Childhood trauma, cognitive biases, neuroticism and depression were all associated with an increased risk of PLEs. Participants who had experienced childhood trauma in conjunction with depression, cognitive biases, or neuroticism exhibited a significantly higher risk of endorsing PLEs compared to those who had solely encountered childhood trauma. Path analysis revealed that cognitive biases, neuroticism and depression are significant mediators of the relationship between childhood trauma and PLEs. The model explained 44.7% of the variance in PLEs.

Conclusions

Our study highlights cognitive biases, neuroticism, and depression as key mediators linking childhood trauma to PLEs, recognizing the complex interplay among these factors is crucial and should be integrated into clinical screening and therapeutic strategies to mitigate the risk of PLEs.

Introduction

Psychotic-like experiences (PLEs) refer to subthreshold psychotic symptoms that are associated with an increased risk of transitioning to psychotic disorders [1, 2]. PLEs are relatively common, particularly in adolescents [3]. Studies have reported a median prevalence of 17% among children aged 9–12 years and 7.5% among adolescents aged 13–18 years across various countries, including Australia, Spain, and Japan [3]. In China, a relatively higher prevalence of 20.7% has been identified among individuals aged 10 to 23.3 years, as compared to other countries [4]. A substantial body of research indicates that PLEs can lead to the development of psychotic disorders [5], non-psychotic disorders [6, 7], as well as an elevated risk of various adverse outcomes, encompassing emotional disturbances, suicidal tendencies and social dysfunction [8,9,10]. Therefore, identifying the influencing factors of PLEs in adolescents is crucial for the early prevention of mental health issues.

PLEs share several risk factors with psychotic disorders, including childhood trauma, socioeconomic status, place of residence, cognitive dysfunctions, depressive symptoms, and individual personality traits [11,12,13,14,15]. Accumulating evidence suggests that individuals with a history of childhood trauma are more likely to experience PLEs [16], with trauma considered a significant factor in subclinical psychotic symptoms [17, 18]. Nonetheless, exposure to traumatic life events alone is insufficient to account for the development of psychosis [19]. Recent studies have proposed that traumatic life events increase the risk of psychosis through cognitive biases, which refer to systematic errors in cognitive processing and irrational judgment [20]. Cognitive biases, supported by the cognitive model of psychosis, may play a critical role in the formation and persistence of psychotic symptoms [21]. According to this model, vulnerable predispositions (e.g., certain personality traits) interact with traumatic life events and adverse environments, leading to emotional changes and cognitive biases that contribute to the development of subthreshold or threshold positive psychotic symptoms, including PLEs [14, 19, 22].

Among personality traits linked to the risk of mental disorders, neuroticism is particularly noteworthy. Neuroticism is elevated in a wide range of psychological disorders, including schizophrenia-spectrum disorders, major depression, and generalized anxiety [23,24,25]. It is characterized by heightened emotional reactivity, specifically a tendency to become easily aroused and to slowly return to emotional baseline [26, 27]. Individuals with high levels of neuroticism are more prone to negative emotions, such as sadness, fear, and anger. Increased neuroticism has been associated with the persistence of early PLEs [15].

Depressive symptoms are also considered significant predictors of PLEs [14]. Depression is highly prevalent among individuals with psychosis, as well as among those at ultra-high risk of developing psychosis [28, 29]. Previous studies have shown that individuals with mood disorders face a fourfold increased risk of experiencing psychotic symptoms [30]. Furthermore, depressive symptoms are associated with an increased likelihood of PLEs in the general population [31, 32].

In summary, PLEs are likely to result from the interplay between childhood trauma and other processes involved in the development of psychosis. Specifically, the combination of childhood trauma, cognitive biases, neuroticism, and depression appears to contribute to the emergence of PLEs. Despite the importance of this topic, relatively few studies have examined the mechanisms by which these factors interact to impact PLEs. The current study aims to investigate how childhood trauma, cognitive biases, neuroticism, and depression jointly influence the risk of PLEs in adolescents. Based on the cognitive model of psychosis and prior research, we hypothesize that childhood trauma increases the risk of PLEs through dysfunctional cognitive processes (cognitive biases), emotional changes (depression), and personality traits (neuroticism). Specifically, we propose that childhood trauma alters cognitive processing through neuroticism, which in turn increases the frequency of PLEs, and that childhood trauma also influences mood, leading to depressive symptoms and a heightened risk of PLEs.

Methods

Participants

A convenience sample of adolescents was recruited from three senior high schools in Hunan Province, China. Participants with a history of psychiatric disorders or neurological conditions, based on self-reported data, were excluded from the analysis (98 participants excluded). The final sample included 4,087 participants (2,026 females, 2,061 males), aged 14–19 years (M = 16.16, SD = 0.88). This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB) of the First Affiliated Hospital of Hunan University of Chinese Medicine, and written informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Assessments

Demographic data

A sociodemographic and clinical questionnaire was designed to collect basic information, including age, sex, residence, only-child status, economic status, and parental marital status. Economic status was measured using the MacArthur Scale [33], a 10-rung ladder that assesses perceived socioeconomic status, with the top rung representing the highest socioeconomic status and the bottom representing the lowest. This scale has been validated among Chinese adults [34].

Psychotic-like experiences (PLEs)

PLEs were measured using the Community Assessment of Psychic Experiences (CAPE-P15) [35]. The CAPE-P15 consists of two subscales assessing the frequency and distress of psychotic-like experiences across 15 items. These items are categorized into three factors: persecutory ideation (PI), bizarre experiences (BE), and perceptual abnormalities (PA). A mean cut-off score of 1.47 on both subscales (frequency and distress) was used to screen for ultra-high risk of psychosis [36]. The Chinese version of the CAPE-P15 has demonstrated satisfactory psychometric properties among adolescents [37], with a Cronbach’s α of 0.896 in this study.

Depression

Depressive symptoms were assessed using the Patient Health Questionnaire-9 (PHQ-9), a widely used tool for screening and evaluating the severity of depression [38]. The PHQ-9 consists of nine items rated on a scale of 0 to 3, with total scores ranging from 0 to 27. In this study, a cut-off score of 15 was adopted to identify positive cases. The Chinese version of the PHQ-9 has demonstrated good psychometric properties [39], with a Cronbach’s α of 0.891.

Neuroticism

Neuroticism was measured using the neuroticism subscale of the Neuroticism-Extraversion-Openness Personality Inventory (NEO-PI). This subscale includes 12 items rated on a 5-point Likert scale, with scores ranging from 5 to 60. Higher scores indicate higher levels of neuroticism. The subscale includes six facets: anxiety, angry-hostility, depression, self-consciousness, impulsiveness, and vulnerability. The NEO-PI has been validated for use in Chinese populations [40], and the Cronbach’s α for the neuroticism subscale in this study was 0.799.

Cognitive biases

Cognitive biases were assessed using the Davos Assessment of Cognitive Biases Scale (DACOBS) [41]. The DACOBS contains 42 items rated on a 7-point Likert scale, with higher scores indicating more pronounced cognitive biases. The scale includes seven subscales: jumping to conclusions, belief inflexibility, attention to threat bias, external attribution bias, social cognition problems, subjective cognitive problems, and safety behaviors. The Cronbach’s α for the DACOBS was 0.921 in this sample.

Childhood trauma

To measure childhood trauma experience before their 16 years old, we the short form of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ-SF) [42]. The CTQ-SF is a 28-item questionnaire using a seven-point Likert scale (1–5). Higher total scores indicate more childhood trauma experienced. Of the 28 items, 25 items are included in 5 subscales (emotional abuse, physical abuse, sexual abuse, emotional neglect, and physical neglect). The 3 remaining items are used to test the validity of each questionnaire. This scale is considered to be a sensitive and valid screening questionnaire for childhood trauma. Four subscales (emotional abuse, physical abuse, emotional neglect, and physical neglect) were used in this study, with a Cronbach’s α of 0.697.

Data analysis

All analyses were conducted with SPSS 26.0 and the AMOS 26.0. First, we conducted a series of univariate logistic regression analyses to investigate whether the variables made an independent contribution to PLE. We adopted a mean score cut-off for ultra-high risk for psychosis of 1.47 for both frequency and distress scores on CAPE-P15 [36]. Our variables were: Depression, cognitive biases, neuroticism and childhood trauma. With regard to cognitive biases and neuroticism, we divided participants into two groups using a cut-off of 2 S.D. from the mean. Those who scored at least 195.75 points (with 294 as the highest) on the DACOBS were classified as endorsing cognitive biases, and who scored at least 51.13 points (with 60 as the highest) on the neuroticism subscale of the NEO-PI were classified as showing neuroticism. To control for potential confounders, we included age, gender, residence, single child status, economic status and parental marital status in regression models. Then multivariate logistic regressions were carried out using all predictors included in univariate analyses. We also performed stratified logistic regression analysis to identify variables that affect the association between these variables and PLE in different subgroups.

In the next step, we performed confirmatory factor analyses (CFA) of the latent variables. In cases where the original structure of the variables did not reach satisfactory model fit, we modified them by excluding unsatisfactory items. Path analysis was conducted with continuous variables, and for the outcome variable we used the CAPE-P15 frequency score. We also examined the potential multicollinearity of all variables using the variance inflation factor (VIF) [43] indicator. Goodness of model fit was verified following guidelines from literature [44]: RMSEA < 0.06 (The Root Mean Square Error of Approximation); SRMR < 0.08 (The Standardized Root Mean Square Residual); CFI ≥ 0.90 (Confirmatory Fit Index) and GFI ≥ 0.90 (Goodness-of-Fit).

Results

Characteristics of the sample

Table 1 presents sample characteristics.

Table 1 Sample characteristics

Logistic regression analyses

Univariate regression models showed that all predictors were statistically significant. People with higher level of neuroticism had the highest risk of PLEs (OR 14.61, 95%CI 8.94–23.90, p < 0.001). Depression also significantly increased the likelihood of PLEs (OR 12.06, 95%CI 9.23–15.76, p < 0.001), as well as childhood trauma (OR 3.08, 95%CI 2.59–3.65, p < 0.001) and cognitive biases (OR 6.36, 95%CI 4.16–9.74, p < 0.001). Multivariate regression analyses showed that all predictors remained significant in the model for PLEs (all p-values < 0.001). Detailed results are presented in Table 2.

Table 2 OR from univariate and multivariate logistic regressions predicting psychotic-like experiences

Stratified logistic regression analysis were further performed to identify variables that may modify the association between predictors and PLEs (Table 3). Results showed that, compared to individuals who solely endured childhood trauma, the risk of PLEs increased significantly among those with childhood trauma when it was combined with neuroticism (OR 8.94, 95% CI 5.23–15.27, p < 0.001), depression (OR 9.02, 95%CI 6.63–12.27, p < 0.001) or cognitive biases (OR 4.86, 95%CI 2.91–8.13, p < 0.001).

Table 3 Interaction effects between childhood trauma, cognitive biases, neuroticism and depression

Path analyses

Confirmatory factor analyses showed a good fit for all scales except for DACOBS and Neuroticism, thus we excluded six low-loading items (1, 12, 25, 26, 27, 30) of DACOBS and three low-loading items (1, 4, 7) of Neuroticism to reach satisfactory model fit (Supplementary Table S1). Then we tested the hypothesized model to examine associations among childhood trauma, neuroticism, depression, cognitive biases and PLEs. There was no sign of multicollinearity as indicated by the VIF of all predictors < 1.5. The pathway linking childhood trauma to cognitive biases was found to be insignificant and was therefore excluded to achieve the most parsimonious model with a good fit. The final model explained 44.7% of the variance in PLEs. All of the model fit indices were satisfactory: RMSEA = 0.017 (90% CI 0.000–0.049), CFI = 1.000, SRMR = 0.005, GFI = 1.000. The model with standardized path coefficients is presented in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Model of the relationship between childhood trauma and psychotic-like experiences with neuroticism, depression, and cognitive biases as mediators. β = standardized regression coefficients, e = residual errors, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001

Childhood trauma had a significant standardized indirect effect on PLEs through all the other variables (β = 0.251, 95% CI 0.231–0.269, p < 0.05). To find out if the mediation involving cognitive biases, PLEs, and depression was full or partial, we checked for a standardized direct effect of childhood traumatic life events on PLEs. This standardized direct effect was significant (β = 0.177, 95% CI 0.147–0.209, p < 0.05), meaning that the mediation by the other variables was only partial. The standardized total effect of childhood trauma on PLEs was also statistically significant (β = 0.428, 95% CI 0.400–0.454, p < 0.05).

The bootstrapping analysis showed that depression had the biggest impact on PLEs, with a standardized total effect size of 0.490 (comprising a direct effect of 0.445 and an indirect effect of 0.045). Childhood trauma was the next biggest factor, with a standardized total effect size of 0.428 (consisting of a direct effect of 0.177 and an indirect effect of 0.251). Neuroticism also played an important role, with a standardized total effect size of 0.364, which was mostly indirect (0.318) with a small direct effect (0.045). Cognitive biases had a total effect size of 0.166, which was entirely direct. All these variables had a significant overall effect on PLEs. A detailed breakdown of each factor’s effect size was shown in Table 4.

Table 4 Effect analysis of related influencing factors for psychotic-like experiences in adolescents. (n = 4087)

Discussion

In the current study, we reported a prevalence rate of 20.6% for PLE among adolescents, which is consistent with one prior study conducted in China [4]. And we further confirmed that the risk of PLEs results from the interplay between childhood trauma, cognitive biases, neuroticism and depression among adolescents. In line with previous clinical and non-clinical studies [18, 45, 46], our findings suggest a pathway from childhood traumatic life events to psychosis proneness. For the first time, our findings revealed that the paths from childhood trauma to PLEs among adolescents go through important, but partial mediators– cognitive biases, neuroticism and depression, it is consistent with the cognitive model of psychosis.

Although previous studies confirmed the role of childhood trauma, cognitive biases, neuroticism and depression in PLEs, this study considered these predictors in combination for the first time. The non-adjusted odds ratio (OR) for the association between the markers under consideration and PLEs decreased in the multivariate logistic regression, after controlling for all predictors included in the analysis and adjusting for age, gender, residence, single-child status, economic status, and parental marital status. However, childhood trauma, cognitive biases, neuroticism, and depression remained significant influencing factors of PLEs. Therefore, our findings suggest that these markers each contribute meaningfully and independently to PLEs in the non-clinical adolescent population.

Even though multivariate logistic regression provided results suggesting an independent contribution of these markers to PLEs, it did not shed light on their potential interactive effect. Our subsequent analysis of interaction between these markers revealed that the combination of childhood trauma with neuroticism, depression, or cognitive bias had a stronger impact on PLEs than childhood trauma alone. For instance, among people who had experienced childhood trauma combined with depression or neuroticism, the risk of PLEs was increased by approximately nine times compared to those who experienced only childhood trauma. Similarly, for individuals with childhood trauma and cognitive biases, the risk of PLEs was approximately 4.86 times higher compared to those without cognitive biases. These findings strongly suggests that these important markers should be considered in combination during screening for risk of PLEs.

Despite progress in researching the mechanisms responsible for PLEs, little is known about how these potential markers might interact in impact PLEs. This study aims to provide a theoretical model that recognizes the complexity of PLEs, extending existing knowledge by demonstrating that the risk of PLEs is largely explained (44.7% of the variance) by the interplay between childhood trauma, cognitive biases, neuroticism, and depression. These findings are consistent with prior research highlighting the interconnectedness of environmental risks (e.g., exposure to traumatic life events), cognitive biases, and depression, all of which have been linked to the risk of psychosis and PLEs [45, 47,48,49]. The cognitive model of psychosis posits that exposure to traumatic events alters cognitive architecture, leading to dysfunctional responses to environmental stressors [50]. Recent studies suggest that cognitive biases significantly mediate the relationship between traumatic life events and psychosis risk in both clinical and non-clinical populations [45, 46]. Additionally, personality traits such as neuroticism are associated with psychosis proneness in both the general and clinical high-risk populations [51,52,53,54]. Consequently, this study supports the view that a combination of childhood trauma, neuroticism, cognitive biases, and depression provides a robust influential framework for PLEs and may be of particular interest for clinical screening and future research.

Actually, most individuals who suffer from PLEs achieve remission, with only approximately 7% of those with PLEs developing into full-blown psychosis [48]. Identifying high-risk factors associated with PLEs is crucial for detecting the risk of psychosis and preventing the onset of mental disorders. Our findings indicate that considering the intricate interplay between childhood trauma, neuroticism, cognitive biases, and depression could enhance the effectiveness of psychosis screening procedures, where a comprehensive assessment of all markers is essential. Regarding the application of our results to therapeutic interventions, it is noteworthy that cognitive biases and depression have already been successfully managed through Cognitive Behavioral Therapy (CBT) protocols [55, 56]. This study suggests that CBT targeting cognitive biases and depressive symptoms could be beneficial in reducing the risk of psychosis in adolescents who have a history of childhood trauma, particularly within school-based health care programs for adolescents.

The interpretation of our results should take into consideration several limitations. Firstly, our sample was restricted to adolescents, potentially limiting the generalizability of our findings to the broader population. Secondly, our study adopted a cross-sectional design and utilized structural equation modeling to investigate the interplay between childhood trauma, depression, neuroticism, and cognitive biases in predicting PLEs. Although these findings provide valuable insights into the theoretical relationships among the studied markers, future research would greatly benefit from employing a longitudinal or follow-up design to further explore these relationships and establish empirical evidence of causality. Additionally, we did not account for the influence of a broader range of mood symptoms and cognitive biases, such as mania, negative symptoms, or jumping to conclusions, which are known predictors of PLEs [10, 57]. Our methods should be regarded as a general estimation of these markers, similar to those used in previous studies [45]. In the path analysis, we excluded several items with low loading from our structural equation modeling, which is an important consideration when interpreting the results. Furthermore, due to the cultural reluctance among Chinese people to discuss topics related to sexuality, questionnaires on sexual abuse were not incorporated into this study. However, sexual abuse, a pivotal aspect of childhood trauma and identified as a risk factor for the development of PLEs in prior researches [45, 58], should be given consideration in future investigative endeavors. Moreover, while this study measured PLEs using a self-report scale, future studies could benefit from incorporating clinical interviews to identify high-risk individuals and obtain more detailed clinical information. And various psychosocial factors, including sleep problems, suicidality, cannabis use, and perceived stress, have been established as significant contributors to PLEs [59,60,61,62]. It is crucial for future research to delve deeper into additional psychosocial factors to gain a more comprehensive understanding of PLEs.

In conclusion, our results suggest that neuroticism, cognitive biases, and depression play crucial roles in the relationship between childhood trauma and PLEs. These findings indicate a clear interactive effect of these markers on PLEs among adolescents.

Data availability

No datasets were generated or analysed during the current study.

References

  1. Fusar-Poli P, Bonoldi I, Yung AR, Borgwardt S, Kempton MJ, Valmaggia L, et al. Predicting psychosis: meta-analysis of transition outcomes in individuals at high clinical risk. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2012;69(3):220–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Yung AR, Yung AR, Pan Yuen H, McGorry PD, Phillips LJ, Kelly D, et al. Mapping the onset of psychosis: the comprehensive assessment of At-Risk mental States. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2005;39(11–12):964–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Kelleher I, Connor D, Clarke MC, Devlin N, Harley M, Cannon M. Prevalence of psychotic symptoms in childhood and adolescence: a systematic review and meta-analysis of population-based studies. Psychol Med. 2012;42(9):1857–63.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Sun M, Zhang W, Guo R, Hu A, Li Y, Mwansisya TE, et al. Psychotic-like experiences and correlation with childhood trauma and other socio-demographic factors: A cross-sectional survey in adolescence and early adulthood in China. Psychiatry Res. 2017;255:272–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Lindgren M, Numminen L, Holm M, Therman S, Tuulio-Henriksson A. Psychotic-like experiences of young adults in the general population predict mental disorders. Psychiatry Res. 2022;312:114543.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Wu Z, Liu Z, Zou Z, Wang F, Zhu M, Zhang W, et al. Changes of psychotic-like experiences and their association with anxiety/depression among young adolescents before COVID-19 and after the lockdown in China. Schizophr Res. 2021;237:40–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Yamasaki S, Usami S, Sasaki R, Koike S, Ando S, Kitagawa Y, et al. The association between changes in depression/anxiety and trajectories of psychotic-like experiences over a year in adolescence. Schizophr Res. 2018;195:149–53.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Karcher NR, O’Hare K, Jay SY, Grattan R. Strengthening associations between psychotic like experiences and suicidal ideation and behavior across middle childhood and early adolescence. Psychol Med. 2023;53(13):6002–10.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Deng Wisteria Y, Grove T, Deldin P. Anxiety mediates the relationship between Psychotic-Like experiences and social functioning in the general population. Psychopathology. 2020;53(2):95–102.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Yang X-H, Zhang J-w, Li Y, Zhou L, Sun M. Psychotic-like experiences as a co-occurring psychopathological indicator of multi-dimensional affective symptoms: findings from a cross-sectional survey among college students. J Affect Disord. 2023;323:33–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Scott J, Chant D, Andrews G, Martin G, McGrath J. Association between trauma exposure and delusional experiences in a large community-based sample. Br J Psychiatry. 2007;190(4):339–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Gawęda Ł, Göritz AS, Moritz S. Mediating role of aberrant salience and self-disturbances for the relationship between childhood trauma and psychotic-like experiences in the general population. Schizophr Res. 2019;206:149–56.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Kelleher I, Clarke MC, Rawdon C, Murphy J, Cannon M. Neurocognition in the extended psychosis phenotype: performance of a community sample of adolescents with psychotic symptoms on the MATRICS neurocognitive battery. Schizophr Bull. 2013;39(5):1018–26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Barragan M, Laurens KR, Navarro JB, Obiols JE. Psychotic-like experiences and depressive symptoms in a community sample of adolescents. Eur Psychiatry. 2011;26(6):396–401.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Hafeez D, Yung AR. Early persistence of psychotic-like experiences in a community sample of adolescents. Early Interv Psychiat. 2021;15(6):1575–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Lu D, Wang W, Qiu X, Qing Z, Lin X, Liu F, et al. The prevalence of confirmed childhood trauma and its’ impact on psychotic-like experiences in a sample of Chinese adolescents. Psychiatry Res. 2020;287:112897.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Isvoranu A-M, van Borkulo CD, Boyette L-L, Wigman JTW, Vinkers CH, Borsboom D, et al. A network approach to psychosis: pathways between childhood trauma and psychotic symptoms. Schizophr Bull. 2017;43(1):187–96.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Heins M, Simons C, Lataster T, Pfeifer S, Versmissen D, Lardinois M, et al. Childhood trauma and psychosis: A Case-Control and Case-Sibling comparison across different levels of genetic liability, psychopathology, and type of trauma. Am J Psychiatry. 2011;168(12):1286–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Gibson LE, Alloy LB, Ellman LM. Trauma and the psychosis spectrum: A review of symptom specificity and explanatory mechanisms. Clin Psychol Rev. 2016;49:92–105.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Kotowicz K, Gawęda Ł, Misiak B, Frydecka D. The role of the cognitive biases along the psychosis continuum: from pathophysiology to treatment options. Arch Psychiatry Psychother. 2020;24(1):49–53.

  21. Garety PA, Kuipers E, Fowler D, Freeman D, Bebbington PE. A cognitive model of the positive symptoms of psychosis. Psychol Med. 2001;31(2):189–95.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Howes OD, Murray RM. Schizophrenia: an integrated sociodevelopmental-cognitive model. Lancet. 2014;383(9929):1677–87.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Ormel J, Jeronimus BF, Kotov R, Riese H, Bos EH, Hankin B, et al. Neuroticism and common mental disorders: meaning and utility of a complex relationship. Clin Psychol Rev. 2013;33(5):686–97.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Hettema JM, Neale MC, Myers JM, Prescott CA, Kendler KS. A Population-Based twin study of the relationship between neuroticism and internalizing disorders. Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163(5):857–64.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Ormel J, Oldehinkel AJ, Ferdinand RF, Hartman CA, De Winter AF, Veenstra R, et al. Internalizing and externalizing problems in adolescence: general and dimension-specific effects of Familial loadings and preadolescent temperament traits. Psychol Med. 2005;35(12):1825–35.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Lahey BB. Public health significance of neuroticism. Am Psychol. 2009;64(4):241–56.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Perkins AM, Arnone D, Smallwood J, Mobbs D. Thinking too much: self-generated thought as the engine of neuroticism. Trends Cogn Sci. 2015;19(9):492–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Conley RR, Ascher-Svanum H, Zhu B, Faries DE, Kinon BJ. The burden of depressive symptoms in the long-term treatment of patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2007;90(1):186–97.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Schirmbeck F, van der Burg NC, Blankers M, Vermeulen JM, McGuire P, Valmaggia LR, et al. Impact of comorbid affective disorders on longitudinal clinical outcomes in individuals at Ultra-high risk for psychosis. Schizophr Bull. 2022;48(1):100–10.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Varghese D, Scott J, Welham J, Bor W, Najman J, O’Callaghan M, et al. Psychotic-Like experiences in major depression and anxiety disorders: A Population-Based survey in young adults. Schizophr Bull. 2011;37(2):389–93.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. van Os J, Pries L-K, ten Have M, de Graaf R, van Dorsselaer S, Delespaul P, et al. Evidence, and replication thereof, that molecular-genetic and environmental risks for psychosis impact through an affective pathway. Psychol Med. 2022;52(10):1910–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Hartley S, Barrowclough C, Haddock G. Anxiety and depression in psychosis: a systematic review of associations with positive psychotic symptoms. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2013;128(5):327–46.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Adler NE, Epel ES, Castellazzo G, Ickovics JR. Relationship of subjective and objective social status with psychological and physiological functioning: preliminary data in healthy white women. Health Psychol. 2000;19(6):586–92.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Huang S, Hou J, Sun L, Dou D, Liu X, Zhang H. The effects of objective and subjective socioeconomic status on subjective Well-Being among Rural-to-Urban migrants in China: the moderating role of subjective social mobility. Front Psychol. 2017. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00819

  35. Capra C, Kavanagh DJ, Hides L, Scott J. Brief screening for psychosis-like experiences. Schizophr Res. 2013;149(1):104–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Bukenaite A, Stochl J, Mossaheb N, Schäfer MR, Klier CM, Becker J, et al. Usefulness of the CAPE-P15 for detecting people at ultra-high risk for psychosis: psychometric properties and cut-off values. Schizophr Res. 2017;189:69–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Sun M, Wang D, Jing L, Xi C, Dai L, Zhou L. Psychometric properties of the 15-item positive subscale of the community assessment of psychic experiences. Schizophr Res. 2020;222:160–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, Williams JB. The PHQ-9: validity of a brief depression severity measure. J Gen Intern Med. 2001;16(9):606–13.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Zhang Y-L, Liang W, Chen Z-M, Zhang H-M, Zhang J-H, Weng X-Q, et al. Validity and reliability of patient health Questionnaire-9 and patient health Questionnaire-2 to screen for depression among college students in China. Asia-Pacific Psychiatry. 2013;5(4):268–75.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Xi C, Zhong M, Lei X, Liu Y, Ling Y, Zhu X, et al. Psychometric properties of the Chinese version of the neuroticism subscale of the NEO-PI. Front Psychol. 2018;9:1454.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. van der Gaag M, Schütz C, ten Napel A, Landa Y, Delespaul P, Bak M, et al. Development of the Davos assessment of cognitive biases scale (DACOBS). Schizophr Res. 2013;144(1):63–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. He J, Zhong X, Gao Y, Xiong G, Yao S. Psychometric properties of the Chinese version of the childhood trauma Questionnaire-Short form (CTQ-SF) among undergraduates and depressive patients. Child Abuse Negl. 2019;91:102–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Preacher KJ, Hayes AF. Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behav Res Methods. 2008;40(3):879–91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Lt H, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equation Modeling: Multidisciplinary J. 1999;6(1):1–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Gawęda Ł, Pionke R, Hartmann J, Nelson B, Cechnicki A, Frydecka D. Toward a complex network of risks for psychosis: combining trauma, cognitive biases, depression, and Psychotic-like experiences on a large sample of young adults. Schizophr Bull. 2021;47(2):395–404.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Gawęda Ł, Prochwicz K, Adamczyk P, Frydecka D, Misiak B, Kotowicz K, et al. The role of self-disturbances and cognitive biases in the relationship between traumatic life events and psychosis proneness in a non-clinical sample. Schizophr Res. 2018;193:218–24.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Ames CS, Jolley S, Laurens KR, Maddox L, Corrigall R, Browning S, et al. Modelling psychosocial influences on the distress and impairment caused by psychotic-like experiences in children and adolescents. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2014;23(8):715–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Linscott RJ, van Os J. An updated and conservative systematic review and meta-analysis of epidemiological evidence on psychotic experiences in children and adults: on the pathway from proneness to persistence to dimensional expression across mental disorders. Psychol Med. 2013;43(6):1133–49.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Gin K, Stewart C, Abbott C, Banerjea P, Bracegirdle K, Browning S, et al. Psychosocial predictors of distressing unusual experiences in adolescence: testing the fit of an adult cognitive model of psychosis. Schizophr Res. 2021;237:1–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Garety PA, Bebbington P, Fowler D, Freeman D, Kuipers E. Implications for Neurobiological research of cognitive models of psychosis: a theoretical paper. Psychol Med. 2007;37(10):1377–91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Wiltink S, Nelson B, Velthorst E, Wigman JTW, Lin A, Baksheev G, et al. The relationship between personality traits and psychotic like experiences in a large non-clinical adolescent sample. Pers Individ Dif. 2015;73:92–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Van Os J, Jones PB. Neuroticism as a risk factor for schizophrenia. Psychol Med. 2001;31(6):1129–34.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Krabbendam L, Janssen I, Bak M, Bijl RV, de Graaf R, van Os J. Neuroticism and low self-esteem as risk factors for psychosis. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2002;37(1):1–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Goodwin RD, Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ. Neuroticism in adolescence and psychotic symptoms in adulthood. Psychol Med. 2003;33(6):1089–97.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Moritz S, Andreou C, Schneider BC, Wittekind CE, Menon M, Balzan RP, et al. Sowing the seeds of doubt: a narrative review on metacognitive training in schizophrenia. Clin Psychol Rev. 2014;34(4):358–66.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Oud M, de Winter L, Vermeulen-Smit E, Bodden D, Nauta M, Stone L, et al. Effectiveness of CBT for children and adolescents with depression: A systematic review and meta-regression analysis. Eur Psychiatry. 2019;57:33–45.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Livet A, Navarri X, Potvin S, Conrod P. Cognitive biases in individuals with psychotic-like experiences: A systematic review and a meta-analysis. Schizophr Res. 2020;222:10–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. O’Neill T, Maguire A, Shevlin M. Sexual trauma in childhood and adulthood as predictors of Psychotic-like experiences: the mediating role of dissociation. Child Abuse Rev. 2021;30(5):431–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Kurkinen K, Kärkkäinen O, Lehto SM, Luoma I, Kraav SL, Kivimäki P, et al. An exploratory study of metabolomics in endogenous and cannabis-use-associated psychotic-like experiences in adolescence. Transl Psychiatry. 2024;14(1):466.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  60. Korenic SA, Ered A, Pierce KM, Calvo EM, Olino TM, Murty VP, et al. Examining self-reported social functioning, sleep quality, and psychotic-like experiences in college students. J Psychiatr Res. 2021;143:54–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  61. Bu LW, Wang DF, Fan YG, Ye HX, Liu WX, Fan F. Sleep disturbance and suicidal ideation mediated by psychotic-like experiences in adolescents: a two-wave longitudinal study. Sleep. 2024;47(3):zsae013.

  62. Ered A, Gibson LE, Maxwell SD, Cooper S, Ellman LM. Coping as a mediator of stress and psychotic-like experiences. Eur Psychiatry. 2017;43:9–13.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors acknowledge all the participants who participated in this study.

Funding

This study was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (No: 62402170), the Natural Science Foundation of Hunan Province, China (No: 2023JJ40489), the Scientific Research Fund of Hunan Provincial Education Department, China (No: 22B0363), the Hunan Provincial Philosophy and Social Science Achievement Evaluation Committee (No: XSP2023JYC212), the Social Science Foundation of Jiangsu Province (No: 23SHC011).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Research question conceptualization and study design (all authors). Data acquisition (CX), statistical analysis, and responsibility for integrity and accuracy of data analysis (SW, XX, CX). Interpretation of data (all authors). Drafting of the manuscript (CX, SW). Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content (all authors).

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Chang Xi.

Ethics declarations

Ethical approval and consent to participate

This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB) of the First Affiliated Hospital of Hunan University of Chinese Medicine, and written informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary Material 1

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Xi, C., Xu, X. & Wang, S. Predicting psychotic-like experiences among adolescents: the interplay of childhood trauma, cognitive biases, neuroticism, and depression. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health 19, 20 (2025). https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s13034-025-00878-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s13034-025-00878-5