You are viewing the site in preview mode

Skip to main content

Heterogeneity of early-onset conduct problems: assessing different profiles, predictors and outcomes across childhood

Abstract

Background

Among early-onset conduct problems (CP), associated with more disruptive behaviors of greater intensity and stability, several domains have been proposed from a variable-centered perspective to capture their heterogeneity: oppositional defiant disorder (ODD), attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), and callous-unemotional traits (CU). Using a person-centered approach, the present study aims to identify distinct profiles of child CP, examining different predictors and developmental outcomes.

Methods

Data included parent- and teacher-reported questionnaires from an ongoing longitudinal study (ELISA). Latent profiles were identified first in a community sample (n = 2,103; age 4–8 years; 50.9% boys) and replicated in a high-CP subsample (n = 168; 70.24% boys).

Results

Four profiles emerged in the community sample (Normative Development, Daring/Impulsive, Low prosociality + Fear; Low prosociality + Psychopathic Traits [PP]), and three in the high-CP sample (same except the normative). The identified CP profiles aligned with the ADHD and CU domains, but not the ODD domain. Differences in activity, punitive and inconsistent parenting emerge as the most significant predictors. Regarding the outcomes, the Low prosociality + PP profile stands out as the group with the most severe emotional, social and behavioral maladjustment.

Conclusions

These findings highlight the heterogeneity within CP, and the importance of designing specific and tailored interventions for each identified profile.

Introduction

Conduct problems (CP) are conceptualized as a recurring pattern of behavior that breaches the rights of others or age-appropriate norms and rules [1], representing the foremost reason for clinical [2] and psychoeducational assistance [3]. CP impact several critical domains of childhood development, including family and school problems [4, 5], and has consistently served as a long-term predictor of antisocial behavior problems in adolescents and adults [6], mental health problems [7] and school maladjustment [8]. Despite the social [2], legal [9], and economic burden [10] that CP entail, there is still a need to better understand the high heterogeneity and comorbidity associated with this behavioral spectrum.

Heterogeneity of CP: pathways and profiles across childhood

CP can be present very early in life [11, 12] and are manifested in diverse forms that include differences at the phenotypic, etiological and developmental level, exhibiting a heterogeneous landscape across lifespan [13]. Developmentally, several studies have examined different trajectories from early childhood, evidencing at least three or four distinct CP trajectories across development, including normative or stable low, childhood-limited, adolescent-onset and persistent pathways [14, 15].

These results extend the initial classical theories that postulated the existence of two trajectories named early-onset or life-course-persistent, and late-onset or adolescence-limited antisocial behavior [16]. Among these, the early-onset group gathered specific attention, because of its elevated risk for later maladjustment [17] and the significant heterogeneity within this subgroup [18, 19]. In recent years, the role of sex has also started to be considered as a part of this heterogeneity. Thus, although the existence of early-onset CP in girls has been debated [20], recent studies have found girls exhibiting this trajectory [21, 22], but also suggesting that girls tend to desist from CP, particularly in their overt forms [23], earlier than boys [21].

An early approach to understanding the development of serious CP within the early-onset group established two main pathways, represented by two phenotypically distinctive subgroups: one characterized by cognitive and emotional regulation problems leading to impulsive acts of reactive aggression [24], and another characterized by the presence of callous-unemotional (CU) traits, present in a small but high-risk group of children with more severe and stable trajectories of misbehavior [19]. Accordingly, both emotional dysregulation, including irritability as a core temperamental trait, and CU traits, considered as the extreme pole of low prosociality, were underscored as key indicators of CP heterogeneity [25, 26], and proved usefulness in the identification of different profiles of child CP [27]. Expanding this distinction, Waller et al. [28] postulated a more comprehensive proposal identifying three different domains characterized by oppositional (ODD), attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and CU behaviors, with ODD and CU domains largely converging with the previous dual conceptualization [24]. These domains could be organized into two distinctive pathways: The “hot” pathway would be characterized by emotional and behavioral dysregulation [18], and it would include children with ODD behavior, exhibiting negative emotionality, elevated internalizing symptoms and anger regulation difficulties [29], and children with ADHD symptomatology, characterized by poor inhibitory control, deficits in attention and impulsivity [30]. The “cold” pathway, on the other hand, would be associated with CU behaviors, marked by uncaring, remorseless behaviors, deficits in empathy, high proactive aggression and deficits in conscience development [31]. This theoretical distinction was supported by studies where items from the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL [33]) formed separate scales of ADHD, ODD and CU behaviors at age 3 [33, 34]. These domains were later validated longitudinally, with CU, ODD and ADHD behaviors showing differential associations with socioemotional, cognitive and behavioral outcomes [28].

Delving into the heterogeneity of CP

Risk and protective factors

Genes and environment act, through multiple individual and contextual mechanisms [35], as a determinant for psychopathology [36]. At the individual level, child temperament is a key factor in the development of CP. The aforementioned multiple pathways to early-onset CP largely converge, at the dispositional level, with the developmental propensity model to CP [37], including oppositional temperament (e.g., negative affect), inhibited harm avoidance (e.g., daring), and low prosociality (e.g., CU traits) as temperamental contributions of antisocial propensity [38]. Relatedly, traits such as impulsivity [39], negative affect, and emotional dysregulation [40, 41] form an undercontrolled profile, associated with both short- [42] and long-term [43] externalizing behaviors. Although these frameworks emphasize some common factors for childhood psychopathology [44] recent studies call for a more nuanced distinction of temperamental traits underlying CP. In this regard, low sensitivity to threat (fearlessness) has been linked specifically to the CU domain [45, 46], while irritability, a core temperamental trait in CP development [26], is particularly associated with the hot pathway and ODD behavior [28, 47].

In addition to these temperamental and individual factors there is another set of variables that have been related to CP across time. Parental practices have been widely studied as predictors of future positive or maladjusted behavior, often in interaction with child characteristics [45]. Parental warmth, the foundation of positive parenting, characterized by a high degree of positive affection, dedication, and a sense of closeness to children [48, 49], has been established as a protective factor against the future occurrence of CP [50, 51]. In contrast, punitive parenting, marked by harshness and punishment, has been linked to the future development of externalizing behavior problems during childhood across several longitudinal studies [52, 53] and, more specifically, with a significant association to the CU group [54]. Currently it is also known that children’s temperamental characteristics can influence parental style, with a notable relationship observed in the dyad fearlessness—ineffective parental practices (i.e., low warmth and harsh punishment) [55], confirming that individual and environmental factors tend to be in constant interaction.

Later outcomes

Although each of the above-mentioned domains (ODD, ADHD and CU behaviors) on their own have sometimes been associated with poor adjustment later in adulthood, when they are combined with high levels CP, the prognosis tends to be worse for multiple types of problem behaviors in adolescence and early adulthood, including violent behavior [56, 57].

Children showing the combination of CU traits and CP (CU + CP profile) usually show worse prognosis later in development, characterized by lower empathy, prosociality and more severe and persistent antisocial behavior [31, 58], bullying [59, 60] and lower academic achievement [61]. However, this combination (CU + CP) has recently been refined by incorporating the full constellation of psychopathic traits (i.e., grandiose-deceitful [GD], CU, and impulsive-need of stimulation [INS] [62]), not only to better inform the heterogeneity of CP, but also to predict more severe CP [63, 64] and other related variables, including ADHD symptoms [65] and aggression [66]. Related to aggression, in school environment, bullying has gained special relevance, associated with externalizing behaviors [67] and with the CU profile especially [59]. The victim profile has been particularly associated with internalizing behaviors [67] but also recently with the CU profile [68].

Current study

Recent studies have emphasized the heterogeneity within the child and youth population, particularly among children with CP. Theoretical frameworks have linked various behaviors to distinct domains (e.g., ODD, ADHD, and CU) demonstrating their relevance from variable-centered approaches [33, 34]. Over time, these associations have been tested in longitudinal studies with clinical [28] and at-risk samples [54]. However, a frequently cited limitation was the need for a person-centered approach, which is particularly beneficial when studying complex and heterogeneous phenomena like CP [69]. Assuming a person-centered approach provides distinct advantages over the variable-centered approach such as the ability to account for individual differences while maintaining some degree of homogeneity, offering greater accuracy and parsimony in capturing the complexities inherent in these behaviors [70]. Starting from this perspective of analysis and using a community sample, the present study is the first to attempt to replicate the existence of the three previously mentioned domains by: (1) Identifying CP profiles based on temperamental and personality variables (e.g., negative emotionality, prosociality, daring, GD, CU, INS, fearlessness) across childhood in both community and high-CP samples; (2) exploring individual and family-level variables that predict profile membership; and (3) examining developmental outcomes related to these profiles. In line with the latest classifications addressing heterogeneity and its associated domains, our main hypothesis was to identify three distinct groups within the CP subsample, following the model of Waller et al. [28]. Similarly, we expected the existence of these three groups within the total sample, anticipating the emergence of a fourth group—normative and majority—given the community-based nature of the sample.

Method

Participants

The present study used data from the longitudinal ELISA project (Estudio Longitudinal para una Infancia Saludable), carried out in Galicia (NW Spain). In this specific research, five waves of the ELISA project, original covering a six-year data collection period, were used to address the different objectives proposed in this study. Parent-reported data from wave 3 (T3: 2019; n = 2,105; 50.9% boys; Mage = 6.10, SD = 0.93, range = 4–8 years) was employed for identifying CP profiles in both the total sample (n = 2,103; two cases deleted due to complete missing data on latent profile indicators) and a high CP subsample (n = 168; 70.24% boys). The High CP group was identified post-hoc from the community-based sample. To identify this subsample, a cut-off point of 1.5 SD was used to determine the presence and intensity of CP above the mean. Children (93.9% Spanish) were attending 72 different schools (76.4% public, 20.8% charter and 2.8% private) located in predominantly working-class communities from different urban, suburban and rural areas. At T3, 82.5% of mothers and 93.3% of fathers were actively working. Additionally, we used parent-reported data from wave 1 (T1: 2017; n = 2.266; Mage = 4.26, SD = 1.02, range = 3–6 years) to examine different temperamental and family predictors, as well as data from wave 4, (T4: 2021; Mage = 8.21, SD = 1.17, range = 6–10 years), wave 5 (T5: 2022; Mage = 7.83, SD = 1.04, range = 7–10 years) and wave 6 (T6: 2023; Mage = 10.24, SD = 1.05, range = 8–12 years) to examine developmental outcomes reported by parents (n = 1,291, 1,630 and 1,343 from T4 to T6, respectively) and teachers (n = 1,426 and 1,675 for T4 and T5 respectively; no teacher-reports were collected in T6). Of note, in 2018 (T2) the initial sample was increased by 361 participants (51.5% boys, aged 3 to 5; Mage = 3.77; SD = 0.87) from a specific area within the same region not covered in T1. As commonly observed in longitudinal studies, attrition across waves was mostly derived from participants’ withdrawal, lack of success in additional contacts for a follow-up participation, by non-returning the questionnaire, or even for mortality or frailty [71].

Instruments

The main informants for this study were parents/caregivers (87.3% mothers). Teachers’ reports were also used to assess certain developmental outcomes. A list of the variables used in the study (see Table S1) along with their descriptive statistics (Table S2) can be consulted in the Appendix.

Latent profile indicators (T3)

Parent-reported

Children’s temperamental variables. Negative emotionality (e.g., “Does your child react intensely when he/she gets upset”; α = 0.77), daring (e.g., “Is he/she daring and adventurous?” α = 0.83) and prosociality (e.g., “Spontaneously shares” α = 0.78) were explored using the Child and Adolescent Dispositions Scale (CADS-P [72]), adapted from Mathesius et al. [73]. This instrument consists of 12 items coded on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (none) to 4 (totally).

Children’s Psychopathic Traits. Children’s psychopathic traits were examined using the Child Problematic Traits Inventory [CPTI] [63]. This instrument consists of 28 items with a 4-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (does not apply at all) to 4 (applies very well). Eight items were used to measure GD (e.g., “Lies often to avoid problems”; α = 0.83), 10 to measure CU (e.g., “Often does not seem to care about what other people feel and think”; α = 0.88) and 10 to measure INS (e.g., “Often has difficulties with awaiting his or her turn”; α = 0.86).

Fearlessness. Six items (e.g., “He/she does not seem to be afraid of anything”; α = 0.87) coded on a 4-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (does not apply at all) to 4 (applies well) were utilized for evaluating fearlessness [63].

Conduct Problems. The Conduct Problems Scale [63], a 10-item measure intended to assess symptoms from ODD and conduct disorder (e.g., “Threatens others”; α = 0.87) was used to identify a subsample of children with High CP (1.5 SD above the mean; n = 168) for replication purposes. Items were rated with a 5-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (almost always).

Additional variables for profile further definition (T3)

Child behavioral variables. Six items for oppositional defiant (e.g., “Disobeys parents”; α = 0.76), seven items for attention deficit/hyperactive (e.g., “Can’t concentrate, can’t pay attention for long”; α = 0.80) and nine items for anxiety problems (e.g., “Worries a lot”; α = 0.70) were used from the Child Behavioral Checklist 6 -18- DSM-Oriented Scales (CBCL [32]).

Predictor variables (T1)

Parent reported

Children’s temperamental variables. Emotionality (five items, e.g., “Cries easily”; α = 0.71), sociability (Four items, e.g., “Likes to be with people”; α = 0.50Footnote 1), shyness (three items, e.g., “Tents to be shy”; α = 0.75) and activity (three items, e.g., “Is off and running as soon as he/she wakes up in the morning”; α = 0.83) were explored using the Spanish adaptation of EAS, Temperament Survey for Children (EAS TS-C [75]). This instrument consists of 15 items from four subscales, coded on a five-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (not characteristic of my child) to 5 (very characteristic of my child).

Social competence. Prosocial/Communication skills (six items, e.g., “Your child listens to others’ points of view”; α = .81) and emotion regulation (six items, e.g., " Your child resolves problems with friends or brothers and sisters on his/her own”; α = 0.80) were evaluated via the FastTrack scale [76] with 12 items coded on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from zero (not at all) to 4 (very well).

Parental warmth. Parental warmth was measured by six items based on the Child Rearing Scale [77]. The items (e.g., “We shared pleasant and loving moments together”; α = 0.82) had a 5-point Likert-type response scale ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (always).

Parent practices. Different parent practices were measured using the Spanish-adapted form of the Alabama Parenting Questionnaire- Preschool revision (APQ-Pr [78]). This instrument has three subscales: positive practices (12 items, e.g., “Friendly talk with your child”; α = 0.75), inconsistent practices (7 items, e.g., “Threaten to punish your child and then do not punish”; α = 0.68) and punitive practices (5 items, e.g., “Spank your child with hand when something wrong”; α = 0.52). The APQ-Pr has 24 items coded on a five-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (always).

Developmental outcomes (T4-T6)

Parent reported (T4-T6)

Children’s CP. Parents rated the aforementioned the Conduct Problems Scale [63] to assess the development of CP in T4 (α = 0.88), T5 (α = 0.87) and T6 (α = 0.86).

Behavioral and psychosocial adjustment. The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ [79]) was used to assess: hyperactivity (e.g., “Restless, overactive, cannot stay still for long”; α = .80, .81, .81, for T4, T5 and T6 respectively), emotional symptoms (e.g., “Often unhappy, downhearted or tearful”; α = .71, .71, .71), peer problems (e.g., “Rather solitary, tends to play alone”; α = .63, .63, .65), and prosocial behavior (e.g., “Considerate of other people’s feelings”; α = .65, .65, .66). Items (five per scale) were coded on a three-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (not true) to 2 (certainly true).

Bullying/victimization. An adaptation with two subscales of Barker et al. [80] was employed. This measure consists of two subscales with four items each: bullying (e.g., “He/she hits or pushes other children”; α = 0.72, 0.67, 0.75) and victimization (e.g., “Hit or pushed by other children”; α = 0.89, 0.89, 0.90). Response options ranged from 1 (never) to 5 (almost always) in a five-point Likert-scale.

Teacher-reported (T4-T5)

Teachers provided information on longitudinal outcomes at T4 and T5. They rated the same aforementioned scales, including scores on CP (α = 0.93, 0.92, at T4 and T5 respectively), hyperactivity (α = 0.86, 0.85), emotional symptoms (α = 0.74, 0.73), peer problems (α = 0.69, 0.71), prosocial behavior (α = 0.77, 0.80), bullying (α = 0.86, 0.84) and victimization (α = 0.85, 0.87).

Procedure

The longitudinal ELISA projectFootnote 2, initiated in 2017, is an ongoing study that has been continuously conducted up to the present day. The research study and procedures were reviewed and approved by Bioethical Committee at the Universidade de Santiago de Compostela: Approval Code: USC-21/2020; Approval Date: June 17th 2016, and November 9th 2020. A convenience sampling approach was used to initially contact 126 schools, of which 72 agreed to participate in the study (public, charter and private). Families were contacted to be invited to participate. After asking for their formal consent, they had one month to complete a questionnaire (paper or online format) at each wave of data collection. Reminders were sent by calls or e-mail by the ELISA staff. The same procedure was followed for teacher respondents. Data collection took place during spring, to ensure that teachers had spent at least six months with children before rating questionnaires. Neither teachers nor parents received any monetary compensation for their participation. Instead, parents and schools were rewarded throughout the different data collection. Schools received educational games for children in T1 and T2. A draw of several sets of books and educational games, valued between EU 50 and 100, was carried out at the end of T3 for both families and schools. At T4, T5 and T6, parents received a report of results about their child’s competencies, with suggestions for improvement, based on their responses to the questionnaires. Additionally, formative talks were offered to teachers and families upon request during all study waves. Confidentiality was ensured through pseudo-anonymity, with each participant assigned a unique ID and alphanumeric code for secure questionnaire access.

Statistical analyses

First, to examine the potential nested nature of the sample, the intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) at the school level was calculated in IBM SPSS Statistics 28.0 for each subscale to be used in the subsequent Latent Profile Analysis (LPA) and to classify participants as High CP. The ICC values were 0.02 or lower, indicating minimal clustering. Since substantial clustering is generally considered when ICC values exceed 0.05 [81], we concluded that accounting for nesting within schools was not required for the analysis of this study.

Second, The Little’s Missingness Completely at Random (MCAR) test revealed that attrition was not missing completely at random, χ2 (236) = 2375.19, p = .025 [82]; yet, the normed test (χ₂ / df) was 1.04, which is below the suggested cut-off of 2, indicated that data was Missing at Random (MAR) [83]. Subsequently, LPA was conducted in Mplus v 7.4 [84] to identify distinct latent profiles of children based on different temperamental and personality variables. To handle missing data, we employed the full information maximum likelihood (FIML) estimator for profile analyses. This method has been shown to provide unbiased parameter estimates, particularly when compared to deletion-based techniques (e.g., listwise deletion) [85], and is especially effective in addressing random data loss and higher rates of missing data [86]. Different models specifying varying numbers of latent profiles were tested. Statistical criteria, along with theoretical and clinical relevance, were used to compare models and identify the optimal number of profiles. Differences in latent profiles indicators were examined with ANOVA in IBM SPSS Statistics 28.0. Subsequently, the Auxiliary option in Mplus was used to compare the best fitting solution on theoretically relevant cross-sectional correlates (BCH method) and early predictors (R3STEP method). This approach was preferred over traditional analyses (e.g., logistic regression) because it allows to examine how auxiliary variables differed across profiles without influencing the classification process whilst takes participant`s partial membership into account [87]. Finally, to test differences on longitudinal outcomes, repeated measures ANOVA with Bonferroni post-hoc test comparison were performed in SPSS. These analyses were conducted analogously, following the same steps in both analyzed samples. First, the analyses were performed on the total sample and then replicated in the High CP subsample.

Results

Model fitting and profile selection

The BIC is a key criterion for comparing latent profile models, with lower values preferred [88]. However, the theoretical interpretability of the profiles is also crucial [89]. For the total sample, although the 5-profile model had lower values of AIC, BIC and ABIC, than the 4-profile model, the LMR test was not significant (p = .364), indicating no significant improvement when increasing the number of latent profiles [90]. Entropy values (better accuracy in classifying individuals into latent profiles) were slightly higher for the 5-profile model (0.91) compared to the 4-profile model (0.89), but the difference was not substantial and still represents accurate classification. As reinforcement for this decision, we have prioritized theoretical interpretability aligning with the idea that a solution with superior statistical fit indices is not meaningful if it lacks theoretical coherence [89]. Hence, the four-class solution better represents the hypothesized profiles, while the addition of another profile did not contribute significantly to the model`s interpretability, as the emergence of a distinctive profile was lacking. Considering both statistical and theoretical criteria, we favored the 4-profile model over the 5-profile solution as it provides a more parsimonious solution to our data.

For the High CP group both statistical fit indices and theoretical/clinical usefulness were also considered to select the best model. Although the LMR test did not indicate a significant improvement with the addition of a third profile, the entropy value (0.76) was similar, and the AIC and BIC indices were lower, suggesting a better balance between fit and complexity [91]. For this group, literature also supports the three-profile model, as it offers a more detailed classification consistent with the complexity of CP-related variables, whilst adding one more profile did not favor interpretability. Therefore, the 3-profile model was chosen over the 4-profile solution due to its greater descriptive clarity, practical relevance, and parsimony [92].

Model fit indices of LPA in total sample and High CP subsample can be found in Table 1.

Table 1 Model fit indices from latent profile analysis (LPA) in total sample and high CP subsample

Differences between profiles (both in total sample and High CP group) on latent profile indicators were confirmed with ANOVA tests (all remained significant at p <.05). Comparisons between groups are shown in Table 2 respectively.

Table 2 Comparisons across TS and high CP profiles on latent profiles indicators (T3)

Representations of identified profile solutions, based on mean standardized z-scores for latent profiles indicators, are shown in Figs. 1 and 2 (for total sample and High CP subsample respectively). Both the Z-scores of the total sample and those of the High CP group were computed in the T3 community sample. Four profiles emerged from the total sample, with the Normative development profile standing for several reasons, including the number of children it encompasses (n = 1,224; 45.9% boys), their significant differences in all variables with all other groups (exception of prosocial behavior which does not differ from the daring-impulsive profile), and its below-average scores in nearly all the variables analyzed (approximately 0.5 SD below), with the sole exception of prosocial behavior, in which this group achieved the highest score. The second largest profile (n = 373; 17.7%; 56.8% boys) was the Daring/Impulsive profile, which, along with the Normative development profile, was the only one to score above the mean in prosocial behavior, with no significant differences between them. This profile stood out especially for a marked daring, with more than 1SD above average. Finally, we found two profiles characterized by low prosociality. First, the Low prosociality + fear profile (n = 349; 16.6%; 57.3% boys), characterized by low scores in prosociality but also low average scores in daring and fearlessness. Of note, no significant differences were observed with the Daring/Impulsive profile on negative emotionality and INS traits. Second, the Low prosociality + PP Profile (n = 157; 7.5%; 61.8% boys) showed the lowest score in prosociality (more than 1SD below average) and significantly higher scores in negative emotionality, psychopathic traits, especially CU traits, and fearlessness compared with all groups.

In the High CP group (n = 168; 70.2% boys), only three profiles emerged: Daring/impulsive (n = 59; 35.1%; 62.7% boys), Low prosociality + fear (n = 71; 42.3%; 74.6% boys); Low prosociality + PP (n = 38; 22.6%; 73.7% boys). In all three groups, as a rule, all the variables analyzed scored above the mean, except for prosocial behavior, with scores below the mean for the low prosociality + fear (1SD) and, remarkably, the Low prosociality + PP (2SD). Low prosociality + PP was the smallest group, but with the highest scores across all variables compared with all remaining groups except for daring and fearlessness (no differences with daring-impulsive profile). Differences between groups on latent profile indicators were not as clear as observed for the total sample. Thus, there were no significant differences between Daring/impulsive and Low prosociality + fear profile on emotional reactivity, GD and CU traits. These groups only remained different in prosociality and INS (lower for Low prosociality + fear group). Differences between Daring/impulsive and Low prosociality + PP were significant for all variables except daring and fearlessness.

Taking both samples into consideration (see Table 2), several observations can be made. First, the Normative development profile (the only one that included more girls than boys) did not emerge in the high CP group, as could be expected. The Low prosociality + PP group clearly manifested in both groups (total sample and High CP subsample) as the most minor, but with more extreme values in the analyzed variables. The Low prosocial + fear profile showed the same trend in both samples, with below-average scores in prosocial behavior, daring, and fearlessness. Finally, the Daring/Impulsive group did not emerge in the same way across both samples (i.e., the values of the variables did not follow exactly the same trends in terms of positive or negative scores, nor in terms of intensity) but both profiles showed a clear shared tendency to score higher in daring, INS traits and fearlessness.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Groups identified using Latent Profile Analysis at T3 (ages 4–8) in total sample (N = 2,103)

Fig. 2
figure 2

Groups identified using Latent Profile Analysis at T3 (ages 4–8) in the High CP group (1.5 SD; N = 168)

Further definition of profiles in behavioral variables (T3)

To better understand the characteristics of each resulting group, several behavioral variables (i.e., ODD, ADHD, and Anxiety; [CBCL [32]]) central in the definition of CP [28] were analyzed (see Table 3). All of them remained significant (p <.05). In the total sample, the normative development group showed the lowest scores across variables. Significantly higher scores in all ODD, ADHD and Anxiety symptoms were observed for the Low prosociality + PP group. No significant differences were observed between the normative development profile and the Daring/Impulsive group in terms of anxiety, nor between the daring/impulsive and Low prosociality + fear groups in ODD.

Table 3 Cross- sectional comparisons between TS and high CP profiles on behavioural variables parent reported (T3)

Overall, for the High CP subsample, higher values were observed across all groups and variables compared to the corresponding groups in the total sample (see Table 3). Notably, the Low prosociality + PP showed again the highest values in all three variables: ODD, ADHD (both followed by the daring/impulsive group), and Anxiety (followed by the Low prosociality + fear group). All groups differed from each other across all variables except for one pair: Low prosociality + fear and Low prosociality + PP in ODD.

Predictor variables for the different profiles (T1)

As predictor variables for classification in each profile, two main blocks were used: variables related to parenting style (i.e., warmth, punitive, inconsistent and positive parenting) and individual variables (i.e., emotionality, sociability, shyness, activity and social competence).

In the total sample (see Table 4), higher levels of activity, punitive parenting, and inconsistent parenting, along with lower sociability, prosocial and communication skills, and emotional regulation, predicted membership in the Low prosociality + PP group compared to the normative development group. This Low prosociality + PP group also differed from the daring/impulsive group in a more punitive parenting style and lower prosocial and communication skills, while higher levels of activity were the only factor distinguishing it from the Low prosociality + fear group. Lower shyness, more activity, and greater prosocial and communication skills predicted membership in the Daring/Impulsive group compared to the Low prosociality + fear group. What differentiated the normative development group from the Daring/Impulsive group was greater emotional regulation, higher shyness, and lower activity. When comparing the Normative development group with the Low prosociality + fear group, we found that better communication skills, less punitive and more parental warmth were the key factors that distinguish membership in the normative group.

Table 4 Probability of belonging to different profiles based on temperamental and parenting variables in total sample (T1)

In the subsample of High CP, the Daring/Impulsive profile was associated with significantly more inconsistent parenting and activity compared to the Low prosociality + fear profile. The remaining variables were not significant in any other group comparisons (see Table 5).

Table 5 Probability of belonging to different profiles based on temperamental and parenting variables in high CP subsample (T1)

Developmental outcomes (T4-T6)

Developmental outcomes of the different profiles have been compared across time considering behavioral and psychosocial adjustment variables (see Table 6 for total sample and Table 7 for High CP subsample). Parent-reported variables showed a clear tendency to be higher than those reported by teachers, except for bullying, which was reported in greater intensity by teachers, especially in total sample. All parent-reported variables remained significant (p <.05) for the total sample, with the Low prosociality + PP group standing out as having the highest levels of CP, social problems, emotional symptoms, hyperactivity, bullying, and victimization, along with the lowest levels of prosocial behavior, although differences between this group and the others emerged as significant just for CP, bullying, and victimization (with highest levels). The opposite trend is observed in the Normative development profile. For teacher-reported variables, the same trend was observed.

Table 6 ANOVA repeated measures for parent- (T4-T6) and teacher- (T4-T5) reported developmental outcomes in total sample
Table 7 ANOVA repeated measures parent (T4-T6) and teacher (T4-T5) reported in high CP subsample

Values reported for the High CP group were also, as a rule, higher than those reported for the total sample group, regardless of whether they were reported by parents or by teachers. Considering parent-reported measures, only hyperactivity levels remained significant, distinguishing only between the Daring/Impulsive and Low prosociality + fear groups, with the latter showing the highest values. In teacher-reported variables, however, notable differences arose, with the Low prosociality + PP profile showing the highest values in social problems, hyperactivity symptoms, and victimization, and the lowest in prosocial behavior.

Discussion

This study explored the heterogeneity of CP throughout childhood, based on relevant variables (individual and family-related) assessed through a longitudinal study. Previous research in this area has typically employed a variable-centered approach [33, 34], confirming the significance of several key domains in conceptualizing heterogeneity in CP: ODD (characterized by high levels of negative emotionality, anger/frustration), ADHD (low effortful control, attention deficits, impulsivity), and CU traits (low guilt and empathy, increased CP and aggression) [28]; each of one associated with specific behavioral manifestations [93]. In this context, the aim of the present study was to evaluate whether a person-centered analytical approach aligns with these findings, including the influence of some predictors and outcomes for each profile.

Identifying resulting profiles

This study analyzed two distinct samples: a broad community-based sample and a subsample characterized by high levels of CP (1.5 SD above the mean). The results identified four distinct profiles in the total sample and three profiles in the High CP group, in line with the hypotheses of heterogeneity in CP. This discrepancy between samples was expected, as the missing group in the High CP subsample corresponds to the normative development group, which includes most of the children from the total sample (58.2%). Among the remaining three groups, the two profiles characterized by low prosociality displayed similar profiling trends across both samples, while the other, Daring/Impulsive, differed in the clustering and intensity of some (psychopathic) traits.

Of the identified groups, two, Daring/Impulsive (characterized by high daring and fearlessness) and Low prosociality + PP (notable for pronounced CU traits and low prosociality) are consistent with the ADHD and CU domains described by Waller et al. [28] in both samples. Furthermore, results for the Low prosociality + PP profile revealed that children in this group exhibited not only high CU traits but also elevated fearlessness and the two additional psychopathic traits: INS and GD [94]. This finding is significant, as it highlights the importance of considering all psychopathic traits—not solely CU traits—for a more accurate understanding of this profile [95, 96]. This group (with all three psychopathic traits) is associated with an increased risk of externalizing problems and stable, long-term antisocial behavior [65].

The third emergent group, Low prosociality + fear, does not align with the established characteristics of the ODD profile. Negative emotionality, a central feature of the ODD domain, was consistently elevated across all three profiles in the High CP subsample. This suggests that negative emotionality may serve as a common trait across CP, supporting some transdiagnostic theoretical frameworks [97, 98]. In this study, the Low Prosociality + fear profile is characterized by low prosocial behavior, heightened fear, low daring, and significant differences in anxiety compared to the other groups. Additionally, this group exhibited elevated psychopathic traits, suggesting a potential link to the acquired CU variant. This variant posits that CU traits may develop through the interaction of environmental factors, with anxiety playing a central role in this process [99].

Regarding gender differences, several key findings emerge. First, while boys and girls are similarly represented in the problematic profiles of the total sample, boys are more frequently found in the profiles within the High CP subsample, particularly in those characterized by low prosociality. This observation is consistent with the trend that boys tend to exhibit higher levels of CP [22], which results in 70.2% of this subsample being boys, leading to an underrepresentation of girls. Future studies should consider gender differences and, when appropriate, include girls who score above their reference group (rather than in the pooled sample) to ensure proper representation. This approach is relevant because, although CP scores are generally higher in boys, a girl with relatively lower CP scores may still exhibit behaviors that deviate significantly from her reference group, which could be problematic [22, 100]. The relatively small gender differences observed in the Daring/Impulsive profile within the High CP subsample (62.7% boys vs. 37.3% girls) could have significant implications that merit further investigation. CP in girls may not primarily manifest as low prosociality but rather through different patterns, such as negative emotionality, daring, impulsivity, and fearlessness. This suggests that, particularly for girls, intervention and treatment strategies should focus on these aspects rather than exclusively on prosociality.

Relevant individual and parenting variables for profile membership

Contrary to expectations, positive parenting and its core element, warmth, do not significantly influence group membership in any profile or sample [50, 51]. However, punitive parenting emerges as a key distinguishing factor in the total sample. While it does not differentiate between the two low prosociality profiles, higher levels predict membership in either of these profiles compared to the normative group. For the Low prosociality + PP profile, higher punitive parenting also differentiates this group from the Daring/Impulsive Profile in the total sample. Similarly, inconsistent parenting predicts membership in the Low Prosociality + PP profile rather than the normative group. This aligns with existing literature underscoring the critical role of punitive and inconsistent parenting in shaping outcomes for this group [52, 54], as these styles are consistently linked to higher levels of CP [31, 101]. Beyond parenting variables, a shared predictor for both low prosociality profiles is a deficit in prosocial and communicative skills, which distinguishes these groups from the normative and daring/impulsive profiles. This finding aligns with research connecting psychopathic traits to socio-communicative impairments [102, 103].

In the High CP subsample, a few variables differentiate between profiles, reflecting the shared risk factors across the externalizing spectrum. Nevertheless, two variables stand out: inconsistent parenting and activity levels. Increased inconsistent parenting predicts membership in the Daring/Impulsive profile compared to the Low prosociality + fear profile, reinforcing the importance of parenting in this group. This finding diverges from some recent studies suggesting less relevance of parenting for Low prosociality + fear group [104]. Activity levels consistently differentiate the Low prosociality + fear profile, characterized by lower levels, from the Daring/Impulsive profile across both samples. This suggests a stronger association with the hyperactivity component of ADHD in the Daring/Impulsive profile [105].

The reciprocal influence between temperamental variables and parenting practices is particularly important, as we know that parenting strategies can moderate the relationship between temperament and CP [106]. Additionally, we also know that certain temperamental characteristics promote parenting styles characterized by harshness [107]. Moreover, specific temperamental traits for each domain, when combined with negative parenting strategies, not only increase the severity of symptoms but also lead to a higher number of CP. For example, low effortful control is amplified in the context of harsh parenting [108]; high surgency combined with negative parenting results in greater symptom severity of ODD [109]; and fearlessness in a harsh parenting context increases CU traits and future CP [110, 111], whereas the opposite trend is found for ODD, with high fear and harsh parenting leading to more ODD symptoms [111].

Developmental outcomes

In general, parents report higher intensity of outcomes, whereas information of teachers identify more significant differences between profiles. More CP [64] and bullying [59] are observed in the Low prosociality + PP profile in the total sample, closely associated with the domains of the “cold pathway” proposed by Waller et al. [28]. Additionally, this group consistently exhibits the highest scores, according to both parent and teacher reports, in peer problems, low prosociality, emotional issues, and hyperactivity. These results confirm it as the profile with the highest risk and poorest prognosis [31].

In the High CP group, fewer behavioral attributes remain significant, which may reflect the shared behavioral characteristics commonly observed within CP groups [44]. Among the significant variables, hyperactivity does not appear to be a distinguishing trait for the Daring/Impulsive compared to the Low prosociality + PP profile, with the latter showing higher levels of hyperactivity. This finding supports the idea that when the Low prosociality + PP profile includes hyperactivity traits, it may represent a more stable and severe behavioral trajectory [112]. Moreover, the Low prosociality + PP profile consistently demonstrates a higher number of peer problems and lower levels of prosocial behavior, reinforcing its distinctive features [113, 114]. Results about victimization highlight the Low prosociality + PP profile as the group with the highest victimization levels, a consistent finding across both samples and reporters. While traditionally associated with bullying behaviors [59], recent research also emphasizes this group’s vulnerability to victimization [68].

Implications

Current findings underscore the heterogeneity inherent to CP, emphasizing the critical importance of considering the distinctive characteristics of each identified profile, as well as their commonalities, which is essential in both theory and practice. Theoretically, it is important to consider this heterogeneity and to pursue further research that accounts for the specific characteristics of each CP profile. To this end, a more systematic framework to promote the integration of phenotypic, etiological and developmental levels of explanation is particularly needed [115]. Practically, it is essential to tailor and implement target psychosocial interventions based on distinct CP profiles. Early intervention is critical, as it leads to the best outcomes [116] and fostering well-being and school adjustment [117]. Even combinations that are more resistant to conventional treatments (high CP and CU traits) should be considered for its worst future prognosis [97]. Without specialized treatment, negative behaviors such as aggression, harm, bullying, and CU traits may become stable over time [54]. Intervention and particularly prevention programs also benefit from targeting shared risk factors underlying different subtypes of CP. For instance, parenting programs remain to be effective in targeting child CP [118], also for more resistant subgroups (i.e., high on CU traits [119]), with proven effectiveness after tailoring their specific needs [120]. It overall underscores the importance of clearly identifying distinctive phenotypical configurations of child CP, accounting for both common and specific underlying mechanisms, which may positively impact the effect of evidence-based applied interventions.

Strengths, limitations and future directions

These results have several limitations. First, the community sample limits the identification of pure clinical constructs, leading to a small CP group with low sample sizes across profiles. Second, some subscales, such as Sociability and Punitive Practices, had moderate-low reliability. Yet, MIC values, less dependent on the number of items, provided additional support for their internal consistency. Also, the EAS is a widely used instrument for assessing sociability, and the APQ-Pr is a well-established measure of punitive practices, both of which have been validated in the Spanish context [78, 121]. Nevertheless, these results should be interpreted with caution as low reliability could undermine the established conclusions. Third, although a multi-informant approach was included for assessing developmental outcomes, the effect of shared method variance should not be discarded as only parent-reported variables were used for latent profile indicators and predictors. Fourth, key variables for profile identification (e.g., irritability, aggression, emotion recognition) were not included, which may have restrained the identification of a more ODD-based group. Finally, sample attrition should be noted as it may affect, to some extent, the results. Even though the causes of attrition were similar than those commonly observed in longitudinal studies [71], when longitudinal research span different developmental periods, the impact of other variables, including SES and different developmental factors (e.g., IQ, executive function, pregnancy and birth problems) should be also considered [122]. Additional efforts to promote participants’ retention and to mitigate the potential impact of attrition should be particularly encouraged. The study’s strengths include its longitudinal design, use of a stringent CP cutoff to select prominent cases, multiple informants (parents and teachers), and the use of a person-centered approach to profile CP groups. Future research should build on these strengths, incorporating children into their own evaluation process. This will provide a clearer understanding of the developmental heterogeneity of child CP. Also, future studies should explore the stability of temperamental and psychopathic trait profiles over time. Future research will also benefit from the inclusion of additional outcomes, relevant in adolescent adjustment (e.g., academic performance, antisocial behavior, criminal activity), that will help to further understand how the different profiles develop across different developmental stages as well as their impact in the long-term. Finally, it would be important to examine the impact of gender on these latent profiles. Although we did not explore this in the current study, related research using the ELISA sample has identified gender-related differences in outcomes associated with the same latent profiles, which should be further investigated (for more details, see [123]).

Conclusions

This manuscript highlights the usefulness of a person-centered approach in analyzing the heterogeneity of CP. Based on a well-established heterogeneity model, which postulates the existence of three distinct domains with specific characteristics—ODD, ADHD, and CU—these domains were tested using a community sample and a High CP subsample. The different profiles identified in both samples align with the ADHD and CU groups previously proposed. Among the key predictors of group membership, different parenting styles stand out (e.g., inconsistent parenting). In contrast, regarding behavioral trajectories, the Low Prosociality + PP Traits profile (aligned with the CU domain) emerges as the most severe in terms of behavioral risk. The identification of these distinct profiles holds significant theoretical and practical implications, fostering further research and improving clinical interventions tailored to the specific characteristics of each profile.

Data availability

Data and materials that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author, B.D.-V., upon request.

Notes

  1. For those variables with poor Cronbach’s alpha (> 0.60), the mean inter-item correlation (MIC) was computed as an additional indicator of internal consistency, with scores ranging 0.15-0.50 being considered adequate [74]. Scales affected were T1 Sociability (MIC = 0.31) and Punitive parenting (MIC = 0.32).

  2. See http://www.personalitydevelopmentcollaborative.org/project-page-elisa/ for more details.

Abbreviations

CP:

Conduct problems

ODD:

Oppositional defiant disorder

ADHD:

Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder

CU:

Callous-unemotional

NW:

Northwest

PP:

Psychopathic traits

CBCL:

Child Behavioral Checklist

GD:

Grandiose-deceitful

INS:

Impulsive-need of stimulation

CADS-P:

Child and Adolescent Dispositions Scale

CPTI:

Child Problematic Traits Inventory

EAS TS-C:

EAS Temperament Survey for Children

APQ-Pr:

Alabama Parenting Questionnaire- Preschool revision

SDQ:

Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire

LPA:

Latent profile analysis

BIC:

Bayesian information criterion

LMR:

Lo-Mendell-Rubin

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5. 5th ed. Arlington (VA): American Psychiatric Association; 2022.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  2. Rivenbark JG, Odgers CL, Caspi A, Harrington H, Hogan S, Houts RM, et al. The high societal costs of childhood conduct problems: evidence from administrative records up to age 38 in a longitudinal birth cohort. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2018;59(6):703–10. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1111/jcpp.12850.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Merikangas KR, Nakamura EF, Kessler RC. Epidemiology of mental disorders in children and adolescents. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2009;11(1):7–20. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.31887/DCNS.2009.11.1/krmerikangas.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Lau MA, Temcheff CE, Poirier M, Commisso M, Déry M. Longitudinal relationships between conduct problems, depressive symptoms, and school dropout. J Sch Psychol. 2023;96:12–23. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.jsp.2022.10.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Wang MT, Degol JL, Amemiya J, Parr A, Guo J. Classroom climate and children’s academic and psychological wellbeing: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Dev Rev. 2020;57:100912. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.dr.2020.100912.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Shaw DS, Gross HE. What we have learned about early childhood and the development of delinquency. In: Liberman AK, editor. The long view of crime: a synthesis of longitudinal research. Springer; 2008. pp. 79–127.

  7. Ferrari AJ, Santomauro DF, Herrera AM, Shadid J, Ashbaugh C, Erskine HE, et al. Global, regional, and National burden of 12 mental disorders in 204 countries and territories, 1990–2019: A systematic analysis for the global burden of disease study 2019. Lancet Psychiatry. 2022;9(2):137–50. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/S2215-0366(21)00395-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Hyland L, Mháille G, Lodge A, McGilloway S. Conduct problems in young, school-going children in Ireland: prevalence and teacher response. School Psychol Int. 2014;35(5):516–29. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1177/0143034313515984.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Bevilacqua L, Hale D, Barker ED, Viner R. Conduct problems trajectories and psychosocial outcomes: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2018;27(10):1239–60. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/s00787-017-1053-4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Goulter N, Hur YS, Jones DE, Godwin J, McMahon RJ, Dodge KA, et al. Kindergarten conduct problems are associated with monetized outcomes in adolescence and adulthood. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip. 2024;65(3):328–39. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1111/jcpp.13837.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Lorber MF, Del Vecchio T, Slep AMS. The emergence and evolution of infant externalizing behavior. Dev Psychopathol. 2015;27(3):663–80. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1017/S0954579414000923.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Scheper FY, Majdandžić M, Van de Ven PM, Jansen LC, Doreleijers TA, Schuengel C, de Vries ALC. Temperament traits and psychopathology in young clinically referred children compared to a general population sample. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2017;48(6):841–50. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/s10578-016-0708-6.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. McMahon RJ, Frick PJ. Evidence-Based assessment of conduct problems in children and adolescents. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2005;34:477–505. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1207/s15374424jccp3403_6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. López-Romero L, Romero E, Andershed H. Conduct problems in childhood and adolescence: developmental trajectories, predictors and outcomes in a six-year follow up. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2015;46(5):762–73. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/s10578-014-0518-7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Sentse M, Kretschmer T, de Haan A, Prinzie P. Conduct problem trajectories between age 4 and 17 and their association with behavioral adjustment in emerging adulthood. J Youth Adolesc. 2017;46(8):1633–42. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/s10964-016-0476-4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Moffitt TE. Adolescence-limited and life-course-persistent antisocial behavior: A developmental taxonomy. Psychol Rev. 1993;100(4):674–701. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1037/0033-295x.100.4.674.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. López-Romero L, Romero E, Villar P. Developmental trajectories of youth conduct problems: testing later development and related outcomes in a 12-year period. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2017;48(4):619–31. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/s10578-016-0686-8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Frick PJ. Developmental pathways to conduct disorder: implications for future directions in research, assessment, and treatment. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2012;41(3):378–89. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1080/15374416.2012.664815.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Frick PJ, Viding E. Antisocial behavior from a developmental psychopathology perspective. Dev Psychopathol. 2009;21(4):1111–31. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1017/S0954579409990071.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Silverthorn P, Frick PJ. Developmental pathways to antisocial behavior: the delayed-onset pathway in girls. Dev Psychopathol. 1999;11(1):101–26. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1017/S0954579499001972.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Álvarez-Voces M, Romero E. Gender differences in the development of children’s conduct problems: A Four-Year longitudinal study. J Child Fam Stud. 2025. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/s10826-024-02999-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Gutman LM, Joshi H, Parsonage M, Schoon I. Gender-specific trajectories of conduct problems from ages 3 to 11. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2018;46(7):1467–80. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/s10802-017-0379-1.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Eliot L. Brain development and physical aggression: how a small gender difference grows into a violence problem. Curr Anthropol. 2021;62(Suppl 23):S66–78. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1086/711705.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Pardini D, Frick PJ. Multiple developmental pathways to conduct disorder: current conceptualizations and clinical implications. J Can Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2013;22(1):20–5.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Hung T, Viding E, Stringaris A, Ganiban JM, Saudino KJ. Understanding the etiology of externalizing problems in young children: the roles of callous-unemotional traits and irritability. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2025. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.jaac.2025.01.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Wakschlag LS, Perlman SB, Blair RJ, Leibenluft E, Briggs-Gowan MJ, Pine DS. The neurodevelopmental basis of early childhood disruptive behavior: irritable and callous phenotypes as exemplars. Am J Psychiatry. 2018;175(2):114–30. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1176/appi.ajp.2017.1701004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Fanti KA, Kimonis E. Heterogeneity in externalizing problems at age 3: association with age 15 biological and environmental outcomes. Dev Psychol. 2017;53(7):1230–1241. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1037/dev0000317.

  28. Waller R, Hyde L, Grabell A, Alves M, Olson S. Differential associations of early callous-unemotional, oppositional, and ADHD behaviors: multiple domains within early-starting conduct problems? J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2015;56(6):657–66. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1111/jcpp.12326.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Stringaris A, Goodman R. Longitudinal outcome of youth oppositionality: irritable, headstrong, and hurtful behaviors have distinctive predictions. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2009;48(4):404–12. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181984f30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. von Stauffenberg C, Campbell SB. Predicting the early developmental course of symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Appl Dev Psychol. 2007;28(5–6):536–52. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.appdev.2007.06.011.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Frick PJ, Ray JV, Thornton LC, Kahn RE. Can callous-unemotional traits enhance the understanding, diagnosis, and treatment of serious conduct problems in children and adolescents? A comprehensive review. Psychol Bull. 2014;140(1):1–57. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1037/a0033076.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA. Manual for the ASEBA preschool forms and profiles: an integrated system of multi-informant assessment. Burlington (VT): University of Vermont; 2001.

    Google Scholar 

  33. Willoughby MT, Waschbusch DA, Moore GA, Propper CB. Using the ASEBA to screen for callous unemotional traits in early childhood: factor structure, Temporal stability, and utility. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2011;33(1):19–30. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/s10862-010-9195-4.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Willoughby MT, Mills-Koonce WR, Gottfredson NC, Wagner NJ. Measuring callous unemotional behaviors in early childhood: factor structure and the prediction of stable aggression in middle childhood. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2014;36(1):30–42. Erratum in: J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2014;36(1):43–46. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/s10862-013-9379-9.

  35. Kiff CJ, Lengua LJ, Zalewski M. Nature and nurturing: parenting in the context of child temperament. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. 2011;14(3):251–301. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/s10567-011-0093-4.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Rutter M. Understanding and testing risk mechanisms for mental disorders. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip. 2009;50(1–2):44–52. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01976.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Lahey BB, Waldman ID. A developmental propensity model of the origins of conduct problems during childhood and adolescence. In: Lahey BB, Moffitt TE, Caspi A, editors. Causes of conduct disorder and juvenile delinquency. The Guilford Press; 2003. pp. 76–117.

  38. Lahey BB, Waldman ID, McBurnett K. Annotation: the development of antisocial behavior: an integrative causal model. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip. 1999;40(5):669–82. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1111/1469-7610.00484.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  39. Rodenacker K, Hautmann C, Görtz-Dorten A, Döpfner M. Evidence for the trait-impulsivity etiological model in a clinical sample: bifactor structure and its relation to impairment and environmental risk. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2018;46(4):659–69. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/s10802-017-0329-y.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Beauchaine TP, Zisner A. Motivation, emotion regulation, and the latent structure of psychopathology: an integrative and convergent historical perspective. Int J Psychophysiol. 2017;119:108–18. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2016.12.014.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Zastrow BL, Martel MM, Widiger TA. Preschool oppositional defiant disorder: A disorder of negative affect, surgency, and disagreeableness. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2018;47(6):967–77. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1080/15374416.2016.1225504.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Van den Akker AL, Deković M, Prinzie P, Asscher JJ. Toddlers’ temperament profiles: stability and relations to negative and positive parenting. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2010;38(4):485–95. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/s10802-009-9379-0.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. Caspi A. The child is father of the man: personality continuities from childhood to adulthood. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2000;78(1):158–72. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1037/0022-3514.78.1.158.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Dadds MR, Frick PJ. Toward a transdiagnostic model of common and unique processes leading to the major disorders of childhood: the REAL model of attention, responsiveness and learning. Behav Res Ther. 2019;119:103410. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.brat.2019.103410.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Fanti KA, Mavrommatis I, Díaz-Vázquez B, López-Romero L, Romero E, Álvarez-Voces M, et al. Fearlessness as an underlying mechanism leading to conduct problems: testing the INTERFEAR model in a community sample in Spain. Children. 2024;11(5):546. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.3390/children11050546.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. Waller R, Wagner NJ. The sensitivity to threat and affiliative reward (STAR) model and the development of callous-unemotional traits. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2019;107:656–71. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.10.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Burke JD, Boylan K, Rowe R, Duku E, Stepp SD, Hipwell AE, et al. Identifying the irritability dimension of ODD: application of a modified bifactor model across five large community samples of children. J Abnorm Psychol. 2014;123(4):841–51. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1037/a0037898.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Baumrind D. The discipline controversy revisited. Fam Relations. 1996;45(4):405–14. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.2307/585170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Robinson CC, Mandleco B, Olsen SF, Hart CH. Authoritative, authoritarian, and permissive parenting practices: development of a new measure. Psychol Rep. 1995;77(3 Pt 1):819–30. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.2466/pr0.1995.77.3.819.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Kotchick BA, Forehand R. Putting parenting in perspective: A discussion of the contextual factors that shape parenting practices. J Child Fam Stud. 2002;11(3):255–69. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1023/A:1016863921662.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Patterson GR, Fisher PA. Recent developments in our Understanding of parenting: bidirectional effects, causal models, and the search for parsimony. In: Bornstein MH, editor. Handbook of parenting: practical issues in parenting. 2nd ed. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2002. pp. 59–88.

  52. Flouri E, Midouhas E. Environmental adversity and children’s early trajectories of problem behavior: the role of harsh parental discipline. J Fam Psychol. 2017;31(2):234–43. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1037/fam0000258.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Pinquart M. Associations of parenting dimensions and styles with externalizing problems of children and adolescents: an updated meta-analysis. Dev Psychol. 2017;53(5):873–932. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1037/dev0000295.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Waschbusch DA, Baweja R, Babinski DE, Mayes SD, Waxmonsky JG. Irritability and limited prosocial Emotions/Callous-Unemotional traits in Elementary-School-Age children. Behav Ther. 2020;51(2):223–37. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.beth.2019.06.007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Dishion TJ, Patterson GR. The development and ecology of antisocial behavior. In: Cicchetti D, Cohen D, editors. Developmental psychopathology. Risk, disorder, and adaptation. Volume 3. Wiley & Son; 2006. pp. 503–41.

  56. Galán CA, Wang FL, Shaw DS, Forbes EE. Early childhood trajectories of conduct problems and hyperactivity/attention problems: predicting adolescent and adult antisocial behavior and internalizing problems. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2019;49(2):200–14. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1080/15374416.2018.1534206.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Leadbeater BJ, Merrin GJ, Contreras A, Ames ME. Trajectories of oppositional defiant disorder severity from adolescence to young adulthood and substance use, mental health, and behavioral problems. J Can Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2023;32(4):224–35.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  58. Waller R, Wagner NJ, Barstead MG, Subar A, Petersen JL, Hyde JS, Hyde LW. A meta-analysis of the associations between callous-unemotional traits and empathy, prosociality, and guilt. Clin Psychol Rev. 2020;75:101809. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.cpr.2019.101809.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Geel M, Toprak F, Goemans A, Zwaanswijk W, Vedder P. Are youth psychopathic traits related to bullying?? Meta-analyses on Callous-Unemotional traits, narcissism, and impulsivity. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2016;48(6):768–77. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/s10578-016-0701-0.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  60. Viding E, Simmonds E, Petrides KV, Frederickson N. The contribution of callous-unemotional traits and conduct problems to bullying in early adolescence. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip. 2009;50(4):471–81. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.02012.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Ciucci E, Baroncelli A, Franchi M, Golmaryami F, Frick P. The association between callous-unemotional traits and behavioral and academic adjustment in children: further validation of the inventory of Callous-Unemotional traits. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2014;36:189–200. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/S10862-013-9384-Z.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Colins OF, Andershed H, Frogner L, López-Romero L, Veen V, Andershed A-K. A new measure to assess psychopathic personality in children: the child problematic traits inventory. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2014;36:4–21. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/s10862-013-9385-y.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Andershed H, Colins O, Salekin R, Lordos A, Kyranides M, Fanti K. Callous-unemotional traits only versus the multidimensional psychopathy construct as predictors of various antisocial outcomes during early adolescence. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2018;40:16–25. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/s10862-018-9659-5.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  64. López-Romero L, Colins O, Fanti K, Salekin R, Romero E, Andershed H. Testing the predictive and incremental validity of callous-unemotional traits versus the multidimensional psychopathy construct in preschool children. J Crim Justice. 2020;101744. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2020.101744.

  65. Frogner L, Andershed A, Andershed H. Psychopathic personality works better than CU traits for predicting fearlessness and ADHD symptoms among children with conduct problems. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2018;40:26–39. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/s10862-018-9651-0.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  66. Thornton LC, Frick PJ, Crapanzano AM, Terranova AM. The incremental utility of callous-unemotional traits and conduct problems in predicting aggression and bullying in a community sample of boys and girls. Psychol Assess. 2013;25(2):366–78. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1037/a0031153.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Camerini AL, Marciano L, Carrara A, Schulz PJ. Cyberbullying perpetration and victimization among children and adolescents: a systematic review of longitudinal studies. Telemat Inf. 2020;101362. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.tele.2020.101362.

  68. Fontaine NMG, Hanscombe KB, Berg MT, McCrory E, Viding E. Trajectories of callous-unemotional traits in childhood predict different forms of peer victimization in adolescence. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2018;47(3):458–66. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1080/15374416.2015.1105139.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Andrade BF, Sorge GB, Na JJ, Wharton-Shukster E. Clinical profiles of children with disruptive behaviors based on the severity of their conduct problems, Callous-Unemotional traits and emotional difficulties. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2015;46(4):567–76. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/s10578-014-0497-8.

  70. Scrucca L, Saqr M, López-Pernas S, Murphy K. An introduction and tutorial to model-based clustering in education via Gaussian mixture modelling. ArXiv. 2023. http://arxiv.org/abs/2306.06219.

  71. Young AF, Powers JR, Bell SL. Attrition in longitudinal studies: who do you lose? Aust N. Z J Public Health. 2006;30(4):353–61. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1111/j.1467-842x.2006.tb00849.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. Lahey BB, Applegate B, Chronis AM, Jones HA, Williams SH, Loney J, Waldman ID. Psychometric characteristics of a measure of emotional dispositions developed to test a developmental propensity model of conduct disorder. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2008;37(5):794–807. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1080/15374410802359635.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  73. Mathesius J, Lussier P, Corrado R. Child and adolescent disposition model: an examination of the temperament factor structure within early childhood. Int J Offender Ther Comp Criminol. 2017;61(13):1500–26. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1177/0306624X16672931.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Clark LA, Watson D. Constructing validity: New developments in creating objective measuring instruments. Psychol Assess. 2019; 31(12):1412–1427. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1037/pas0000626.

  75. Bascarán MT, Jover M, Llácer B, Carot JM, Sanjuan J. Adaptación Española Del EAS temperament survey Para La evaluación Del Temperamento infantil. Psicothema. 2011;23(1):160–5. https://reunido.uniovi.es/index.php/PST/article/view/9068.

    Google Scholar 

  76. Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group. Social competence scale (Parent Version). Pennsylvania State University; 1995.

  77. Paterson G, Sanson A. The association of behavioural adjustment to temperament, parenting and family characteristics among 5-year-old children. Soc Dev. 1999;8(3):293–309. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1111/1467-9507.00097.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  78. De la Osa N, Granero R, Penelo E, Domènech JM, Ezpeleta L. Psychometric properties of the Alabama parenting Questionnaire-Preschool revision (APQ-Pr) in 3-year-old Spanish preschoolers. J Child Fam Stud. 2014;23(4):776–84. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/s10826-013-9730-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  79. Goodman R. The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: A research note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip. 1997;38(5):581–6. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01545.x.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  80. Barker ED, Boivin M, Brendgen M. Predictive validity and early predictors of peer-victimization trajectories in preschool. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008;65(10):1185–92. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1001/archpsyc.65.10.1185.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Heck RH, Thomas SL, Tabata LN. Multilevel and longitudinal modeling with IBM SPSS. 2nd ed. New York: Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group; 2014.

    Google Scholar 

  82. Little RJ, Rubin DB. Statistical analysis with missing data. 2nd ed. Wiley; 2014.

  83. Ullman JB. Structural equation modeling. In: Tabachnick BG, Fidell LS, editors. Using multivariate statistics. Boston (MA): Pearson Education; 2001.

    Google Scholar 

  84. Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus User’s Guide. 7th ed. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén; 2015.

  85. Lang KM, Little TD. Principled missing data treatments. Prev Sci. 2018;19(3):284–94. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/s11121-016-0644-5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  86. Enders CK, Bandalos DL. The relative performance of full information maximum likelihood Estimation for missing data in structural equation models. Struct Equ Model. 2001;8(3):430–57. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1207/S15328007SEM0803_5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  87. Asparouhov T, Muthén B. Auxiliary variables in mixture modeling: Three-step approaches using Mplus. Struct Equ Model. 2014;21(3):329–41. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1080/10705511.2014.915181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  88. Nylund KL, Asparouhov T, Muthén BO. Deciding on the number of classes in latent class analysis and growth mixture modeling: a Monte Carlo simulation study. Struct Equ Model. 2007;14(4):535–69. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1080/10705510701575396.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  89. Weller BE, Bowen NK, Faubert SJ. Latent class analysis: a guide to best practice. J Black Psychol. 2020;46(4):287–311. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1177/0095798420930932.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  90. Lo Y, Mendell NR, Rubin DB. Testing the number of components in a normal mixture. Biometrika. 2001;88(3):767–78. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1093/biomet/88.3.767.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  91. Burnham KP, Anderson DR. Model selection and inference: a practical information-theoretic approach. 2nd ed. New York: Springer-; 2002. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/b97636.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  92. Lin TH, Dayton CM. Model selection information criteria for Non-Nested latent class models. J Educ Behav Stat. 1997;22:249–64. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.3102/10769986022003249.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  93. Weeland J, Overbeek G, Castro BO, Matthys W. Underlying mechanisms of gene–environment interactions in externalizing behavior: A systematic review and search for theoretical mechanisms. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. 2015;18(4):413–42. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/s10567-015-0196-4.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  94. Klingzell I, Fanti KA, Colins OF, Frogner L, Andershed AK, Andershed H. Early childhood trajectories of conduct problems and callous-unemotional traits: the role of fearlessness and psychopathic personality dimensions. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2016;47(2):236–47. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/s10578-015-0560-0.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  95. Colins OF, López-Romero L, Romero E, Andershed H. The prognostic usefulness of multiple specifiers for subtyping conduct problems in early childhood. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2024;63(4):443–53. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.jaac.2023.05.022.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  96. Salekin RT, Andershed H, Batky BD, Bontemps AP. Are callous unemotional (CU) traits enough?? J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2018;40:1–5. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/S10862-018-9663-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  97. Hur Y, Hwang S, Chung U. Increasing relationship between negative emotionality and conduct problems during childhood: A cross-sectional behavioral genetic analysis. Twin Res Hum Genet. 2015;18(6):785–92. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1017/thg.2015.70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  98. Chin M, Robson DA, Woodbridge H, Hawes DJ. Irritability as a transdiagnostic construct across childhood and adolescence: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. 2025;28(1):101–124. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/s10567-024-00512-4

  99. Kerig PK, Becker SP. From internalizing to externalizing: models of the processes linking PTSD to juvenile delinquency. In: Egan S, editor. Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD): causes, symptoms and treatment. Nova; 2010.

  100. Álvarez-Voces M, Romero E. Gender differences in children’s conduct problems: A multigroup analysis of latent profiles based on temperament and psychopathic traits. Psicothema. 2024;36(1):26–35. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.7334/psicothema2023.44.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  101. Longman T, Hawes D, Kohlhoff J. Callous–Unemotional traits as markers for conduct problem severity in early childhood: A Meta-analysis. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2015;47:326–34. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/s10578-015-0564-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  102. Güney S, Psychopathy. The reflection of severe psychosocial dysfunction. Psychopathy - New updates on an old phenomenon. London: IntechOpen; 2017. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.5772/intechopen.72329.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  103. Vyas K, Jameel L, Channon S. The influence of subclinical psychopathic traits on responding to others’ awkward requests. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2016;21(3):228–41. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1080/13546805.2016.1174681.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  104. Pauli-Pott U, Bauer L, Becker K, Mann C, Müller V, Schloß S. Parental positive regard and expressed emotion—prediction of developing attention deficit, oppositional and callous unemotional problems between preschool and school age. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2020;30(9):1391–400. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/s00787-020-01625-1.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  105. Halperin J, Matier K, Bedi G, Sharma V, Newcorn J. Specificity of inattention, impulsivity, and hyperactivity to the diagnosis of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1992;31(2):190–6. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1097/00004583-199203000-00002.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  106. Belsky J, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van IJzendoorn MH. For better and for worse: differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2007;16(6):300–4. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00525.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  107. Dishion TJ, Patterson GR. The development and ecology of antisocial behavior in children and adolescents. In: Cicchetti D, Cohen DJ, editors. Developmental psychopathology: risk, disorder, and adaptation. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.; 2006. pp. 503–41.

    Google Scholar 

  108. Joseph HM, McKone KM, Molina BSG, Shaw DS. Maternal parenting and toddler temperament: predictors of early school age Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder-Related behaviors. Res Child Adolesc Psychopathol. 2021;49(6):763–73. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/s10802-021-00778-0.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  109. Hare MM, Graziano PA. Treatment response among preschoolers with disruptive behavior disorders: the role of temperament and parenting. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2021;50(6):950–65. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1080/15374416.2020.1846540.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  110. Fanti KA, Mavrommatis I, Díaz-Vázquez B, López-Romero L, Romero E, Álvarez-Voces M, Colins OF, Andershed H, Thomson N. Fearlessness as an underlying mechanism leading to conduct problems: testing the INTERFEAR model in a community sample in Spain. Child (Basel). 2024;11(5):546. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.3390/children11050546.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  111. Waller R, Wagner NJ, Flom M, Ganiban J, Saudino KJ. Fearlessness and low social affiliation as unique developmental precursors of callous-unemotional behaviors in preschoolers. Psychol Med. 2021;51(5):777–85. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1017/S003329171900374X.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  112. Fontaine N, McCrory E, Boivin M, Moffitt TE, Viding E. Predictors and outcomes of joint trajectories of callous-unemotional traits and conduct problems in childhood. J Abnorm Psychol. 2011;120(3):730–42. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1037/a0022620.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  113. Kimonis E, Frick PJ, Barry C. Callous-unemotional traits and delinquent peer affiliation. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2004;72(6):956–66. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1037/0022-006X.72.6.956.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  114. Wagner NJ, Bowker JC, Rubin KH. Associations between Callous-Unemotional traits and Peer-Rated Social-Behavioral outcomes in elementary and middle chool. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2020;48(6):757–69. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/s10802-020-00636-5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  115. Viding E, McCrory E. Disruptive behavior disorders: the challenge of delineating mechanisms in the face of heterogeneity. Am J Psychiatry. 2020;177(9):811–7. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1176/appi.ajp.2020.20070998.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  116. Allen G. Early intervention: next steps an independent report to her Majesty’s government. London: Cabinet Office; 2011.

    Google Scholar 

  117. Chong WH, Huan VS, Quek CL, Yeo LS, Ang RP. Teacher-student relationship: the influence of teacher interpersonal behaviours and perceived beliefs about teachers on the school adjustment of low achieving students in Asian middle schools. Sch Psychol Int. 2010;31:312–28. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1177/0143034310366207.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  118. Romero E, Álvarez-Voces M, Díaz-Vázquez B, López-Romero L. What works in the psychological treatment of child conduct problems? An umbrella review of meta-analytic studies. Rev Psicol Clin Niños Adolesc. 2023;10(1):9–19. https://hdl.handle.net/11000/28825.

    Google Scholar 

  119. Fleming GE, Neo B, Briggs NE, Kaouar S, Frick PJ, Kimonis ER. Parent training adapted to the needs of children with callous–unemotional traits: A randomized controlled trial. Behav Ther. 2022;53(6):1265–81. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.beth.2022.07.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  120. Perlstein S, Hawes SW, Byrd AL, Barzilay R, Gur RE, Laird AR, Waller R. Unique versus shared neural correlates of externalizing psychopathology in late childhood. J Psychopathol Clin Sci. 2024;133(6):477–88. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1037/abn0000923.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  121. Gómez-Ortiz O, Ortiz-Alba M, Falla D, Romera EM. Are Parental Stress and Rewards Influenced by Child Temperament? Analysis of the Moderating Role of Social Support and Gender in Spanish Parents. Psychosoc Interv. 2025;34(1):11–22. doi.10.5093/pi2025a2.

  122. Launes J, Hokkanen L, Laasonen M, Tuulio-Henriksson A, Virta M, Lipsanen J, et al. Attrition in a 30-year follow-up of a perinatal birth risk cohort: factors change with age. PeerJ. 2014;2:e480. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.7717/peerj.480.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  123. Álvarez-Voces M, Díaz-Vázquez B, López-Romero L, Villar P, Romero E. Gender differences in Co-developmental trajectories of internalizing and externalizing problems: A 7-Year longitudinal study from ages 3 to 12. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2024. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/s10578-024-01771-6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank all people involved across the years in ELISA project for their contributions to data collection and analysis. Also, we would like to thank all children, families and teachers for their participation.

Funding

This study was supported by the projects PID2019-107897RB-I00 funded by MICIU/AEI/https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.13039/501100011033, TED2021-130824B-C22 funded by MICIU/AEI /https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.13039/501100011033 and by the UE NextGenerationEU/PRTR, and ED431F (2024/16) funded by Consellería de Educación, Ciencia, Universidades e Formación Profesional (Xunta de Galicia).

In addition, this study was supported by two grants from the University Teacher Training Program FPU22/02200 (BVD) and FPU21/00552 (MAV) funded by MICIU/AEI/https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.13039/501100011033 and by FSE+; and by Grant RYC2021-032890-I funded by MICIU/AEI/https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.13039/501100011033 and the UE “NextGenerationEU”/PRTR (LLR).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

All authors contributed to data collection for the sample used in the analyses. L.L.-R. conceived the study design and developed the statistical framework. B.D.-V. conducted the analyses and wrote the manuscript. M.Á.-V. and E.R. participated in the full review and revision of the draft, approving the final version for publication.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Beatriz Díaz-Vázquez.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval

The research study and procedures were reviewed and approved by Bioethical Committee at the Universidade de Santiago de Compostela. Approval Code: USC-21/2020. Approval Date: June 17th 2016, and November 9th 2020.

Consent for publication

ELISA families were contacted and they were asked for permission to publish their children`s data.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary Material 1: Appendix

. Information related to variables used in the study. This section contains information on the variables used in the study, their main descriptive statistics, their use according to the different waves of time and the informants who reported them.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Díaz-Vázquez, B., Álvarez-Voces, M., Romero, E. et al. Heterogeneity of early-onset conduct problems: assessing different profiles, predictors and outcomes across childhood. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health 19, 43 (2025). https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s13034-025-00897-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s13034-025-00897-2

Keywords